Richard S. Marken The RAND Corporation

Similar documents
(xf - D, yimage) (xf,0) (0,0)

Applying Hooke s Law to Multiple Bungee Cords. Introduction

Interception of a Projectile Using a Human Vision-Based Strategy

Equation 1: F spring = kx. Where F is the force of the spring, k is the spring constant and x is the displacement of the spring. Equation 2: F = mg

How to catch a cricket ball

1. A tendency to roll or heel when turning (a known and typically constant disturbance) 2. Motion induced by surface waves of certain frequencies.

You drop a package from a plane flying at constant speed in a straight line. Without air resistance, the package will:

CHAPTER 1. Knowledge. (a) 8 m/s (b) 10 m/s (c) 12 m/s (d) 14 m/s

Kinematics-Projectiles

Motion, Vectors, and Projectiles Review. Honors Physics

AP Physics 1 - Test 04 - Projectile Motion

ACTIVITY THE MOTION OF PROJECTILES

Fail Operational Controls for an Independent Metering Valve

The effect of back spin on a table tennis ball moving in a viscous fluid.

QUESTION 1. Sketch graphs (on the axes below) to show: (1) the horizontal speed v x of the ball versus time, for the duration of its flight;

Conceptual Questions PM.notebook October 21, Projectile Motion Conceptual Questions

TWO DIMENSIONAL KINEMATICS

SHOT ON GOAL. Name: Football scoring a goal and trigonometry Ian Edwards Luther College Teachers Teaching with Technology

Evidence for a generic interceptive strategy

Page 1. ConcepTest Clicker Questions Chapter 4. Physics, 4 th Edition James S. Walker

Motion is a function of both direction and speed. The direction may

ACTIVITY THE MOTION OF PROJECTILES

(2) An object has an initial speed u and an acceleration a. After time t, its speed is v and it has moved through a distance s.

Performance Characterization of the ABI Cryocooler

The diagram below represents the path of a stunt car that is driven off a cliff, neglecting friction.

Process Dynamics, Operations, and Control Lecture Notes - 20

Practice Test: Vectors and Projectile Motion

PHYSICS 12 NAME: Kinematics and Projectiles Review

Physics P201 D. Baxter/R. Heinz

Lift generation: Some misconceptions and truths about Lift

The purpose of this experiment is to find this acceleration for a puck moving on an inclined air table.

Mathematics in Sports

Low Speed Wind Tunnel Wing Performance

Project 1 Those amazing Red Sox!

Physics 2204 Worksheet 6.5: Graphical Analysis of Non- Uniform Motion D-T GRAPH OF NON-UNIFORM MOTION (ACCELERATING) :

Author s Name Name of the Paper Session. Positioning Committee. Marine Technology Society. DYNAMIC POSITIONING CONFERENCE September 18-19, 2001

Honors/AP Physics 1 Homework Packet #2

Lab Report Outline the Bones of the Story

Regents Exam Practice: Measurement, Kinematics, Free Fall, PJM, and UCM

QUESTION 1. Sketch graphs (on the axes below) to show: (1) the horizontal speed v x of the ball versus time, for the duration of its flight;

Two dimensional kinematics. Projectile Motion

Physics 11 Unit III Practice Test Projectile Motion. Instructions: Pick the best answer available in Part A and Show all your work for Part B

Title: Modeling Crossing Behavior of Drivers and Pedestrians at Uncontrolled Intersections and Mid-block Crossings

Cutnell/Johnson Physics

Physics Acceleration and Projectile Review Guide

TRAFFIC CHARACTERISTICS. Unit I

1. A cannon shoots a clown directly upward with a speed of 20 m/s. What height will the clown reach?

Waves. harmonic wave wave equation one dimensional wave equation principle of wave fronts plane waves law of reflection

PRACTICAL EXPLANATION OF THE EFFECT OF VELOCITY VARIATION IN SHAPED PROJECTILE PAINTBALL MARKERS. Document Authors David Cady & David Williams

1. downward 3. westward 2. upward 4. eastward

TEACHER ANSWER KEY December 10, Projectile Review 1

Characterizers for control loops

Physics 122 Projectile Motion Unit

Fail operational controls for an independent metering valve

Squirt Part VII: cue test machine results

ConcepTest PowerPoints

Principles of Sailing

ScienceDirect. Rebounding strategies in basketball

time v (vertical) time

b. What is the x-distance from the foot of the cliff to the point of impact in the lake?

Behavior under Social Pressure: Empty Italian Stadiums and Referee Bias

Research Article Analysis of Biomechanical Factors in Bend Running

Figure 1: A hockey puck travels to the right in three different cases.

Lesson: Airspeed Control

Gerald D. Anderson. Education Technical Specialist

Boyle s law Verifying the relation between air pressure and volume measuring air pressure in a closed container.

SEMI-SPAN TESTING IN WIND TUNNELS

ROSE-HULMAN INSTITUTE OF TECHNOLOGY Department of Mechanical Engineering. Mini-project 3 Tennis ball launcher

Joint Torque Evaluation of Lower Limbs in Bicycle Pedaling

Contingent Valuation Methods

ITTC Recommended Procedures and Guidelines

Flow in a shock tube

arxiv: v1 [physics.pop-ph] 31 Mar 2008

Chapter 3 Atmospheric Thermodynamics

Kinematic Differences between Set- and Jump-Shot Motions in Basketball

5.1 Introduction. Learning Objectives

HOW FAST/FAR DOES FLY LINE FALL? N. Perkins of the University of Michigan, March 2003

Physics: Principles and Applications, 6e Giancoli Chapter 3 Kinematics in Two Dimensions; Vectors. Conceptual Questions

Putting Report Details: Key and Diagrams: This section provides a visual diagram of the. information is saved in the client s database

Car Following by Optical Parameters

Kinematics Lab #1: Walking the Graphs Results and Discussion. By: Alex Liu Teacher: Mr. Chung Due: October 28, 2010 SPH3U1-01

Ripple Tank Exploring the Properties of Waves Using a Ripple Tank

6 Motion in Two Dimensions BIGIDEA Write the Big Idea for this chapter.

Draw a graph of speed against time on the grid provided.

Development of Fish type Robot based on the Analysis of Swimming Motion of Bluefin Tuna Comparison between Tuna-type Fin and Rectangular Fin -

Educational Methods Using the Passive Walking Paper Robots for Teacher Education

General Physics Physics 101 Test #1 Fall 2018 Friday 9/21/18 Prof. Bob Ekey

Calculation of Trail Usage from Counter Data

End of Chapter Exercises

Biomechanics of Parkour: The Vertical Wall-Run Technique

EMPIRICAL FORMULA OF DISPERSION RELATION OF WAVES IN SEA ICE

DOI /HORIZONS.B P23 UDC : (497.11) PEDESTRIAN CROSSING BEHAVIOUR AT UNSIGNALIZED CROSSINGS 1

Effects of Incentives: Evidence from Major League Baseball. Guy Stevens April 27, 2013

Unsteady Aerodynamic Forces: Experiments, Simulations, and Models. Steve Brunton & Clancy Rowley FAA/JUP Quarterly Meeting April 6, 2011

a. Determine the sprinter's constant acceleration during the first 2 seconds. b. Determine the sprinters velocity after 2 seconds have elapsed.

A SPEED-FLOW MODEL FOR AUSTRIAN -MOTORWAYS

Secondary Physics: The Compass Rose, Cars and Tracks

Helicopter & Launcher

Chapter 10 Gases. Characteristics of Gases. Pressure. The Gas Laws. The Ideal-Gas Equation. Applications of the Ideal-Gas Equation

INTERACTION BETWEEN WIND-DRIVEN AND BUOYANCY-DRIVEN NATURAL VENTILATION Bo Wang, Foster and Partners, London, UK

Transcription:

Fielder s Choice: A Unified Theory of Catching Fly Balls Richard S. Marken The RAND Corporation February 11, 2002 Mailing Address: 1700 Main Street Santa Monica, CA 90407-2138 Phone: 310 393-0411 ext 7971 Fax: 310 451-7063 E-mail: rmarken@rand.org

Abstract. McLeod, Reed and Dienes (2001) argue that there is no unified fielder theory of fly ball catching because there is no theory that can account for a fielder s choice of one path over the many others that will also result in a catch. This path choice criterion is irrelevant, however, if fielders catch fly balls by controlling perceptions of the ball s trajectory. Control theory provides the basis for a unified fielder theory which accounts for the running path data presented by McLeod et al in terms of the perceptions the fielder chooses to control, rather than the path the fielder chooses to take. 2

Many different running paths will get a fielder to the point where a fly ball can be caught. McLeod, Reed and Dienes (2001) argue that a theory of how fielders catch fly balls a unified fielder theory should be able to predict which of these many paths will be the fielder s choice. According to McLeod et al, existing theory can predict the fielder s path choice when the ball is hit directly toward but not when the ball is hit to the side of the fielder. They conclude that a unified fielder theory one that can predict a fielder s path choice regardless of where the ball is hit relative to the fielder still awaits us. Towards a Unified Fielder Theory One promising candidate for unified fielder theory is the linear optical trajectory (LOT) theory proposed by McBeath, Shaffer and Kaiser (1996). LOT theory says that fielders choose a path to the ball that keeps the optical trajectory of the ball linear. Optical trajectory is defined in terms of temporal changes in the tangents of the vertical and lateral projection angles of the ball relative to a fixed reference point, such as home plate. The vertical projection angle, α, is the angular elevation of the ball relative to home plate from the fielder s perspective. The lateral projection angle, β, is the lateral angle between lines connecting the fielder and the ball to home plate. LOT theory says that fielders choose a running path that keeps the relationship between tan (α) and tan (β) linear over time. McLeod et al (2001) tested this prediction of LOT theory and found that for most of the catches they observed the relationship between tan (α) and tan (β) was distinctly curved rather than linear. Moreover, the direction of curvature was the same whether the ball was going over the head or landing in front of the fielder. The curvature results from non-linear changes in tan (β) rather than tan (α), a result that rejects LOT theory but is consistent with the optical acceleration cancellation (OAC) theory of fly ball catching. OAC theory, which is based on the early work of Chapman (1968), says that fielders catch fly balls by keeping the ball s vertical optical velocity time rate of change in tan (α) constant. Constant velocity is equivalent to zero acceleration so OAC theory says that fielders run so as to cancel out or zero the ball s optical acceleration (Tresilian, 1995). But OAC theory is only a partial explanation of the path the fielder chooses in order to get to the ball. It explains path choice when a ball is hit directly at the fielder but it does not explain path choice when the 3

ball is hit to the side of the fielder. McLoad et al argue that a unified fielder theory should explain the fielder s path choice regardless of where the ball is hit relative to the fielder. This path choice criterion is irrelevant, however, if path choice takes place in a closed loop (Marken, 1997). Choosing Paths vs. Choosing Perceptions A closed loop exists when the way a system acts influences what it perceives while what it perceives is influencing the way it acts. The fielder s path choice occurs in a closed loop because the way fielders act (the path chosen) influences what they perceive (the fly ball) while what they perceive is influencing the way they act. When behavior occurs in a closed loop, the behaving system is acting as a control system and the appropriate theoretical framework for understanding the system s behavior is control theory. Control theory shows that the behavior of a control system (such as a fielder) must be understood as a process of controlling perception rather than choosing action (Powers, 1973). A control system acts to bring a perception of some aspect of its environment to a predetermined or reference state while protecting it from the effects of disturbance. This process is called control and the perception that is brought to and maintained in the reference state is called a controlled variable (Powers, 1978). The actions that protect the controlled variable from disturbance are driven by error -- the difference between the reference and actual state of the controlled variable -- not by information about the disturbance itself. So the actions of a control system depend on (often invisible) disturbances to the controlled variable, not on information regarding the actions to be taken to deal with those disturbances. From a control theory perspective, therefore, a unified fielder theory must account for the perceptions a fielder chooses to control rather than the path the fielder chooses to take. Path Choice as the Control of Perception A unified fielder theory based on control theory is described by Marken (2001). The theory explains fly ball catching behavior in terms of control of two perceptual variables: vertical optical velocity and lateral optical displacement. Vertical optical velocity is the time rate of change in the angle of elevation, α, of the ball. The angle α is the angle made by two lines, one connecting the fielder to home plate and the other connecting the ball to home plate. Lateral 4

optical displacement is the lateral (azimuth) angle, γ, between lines connecting the ball and the fielder to a point directly in front of the fielder. The model assumes that the fielder is always looking straight ahead so the angle γ is the angle made by lines connecting the fielder and ball to a point (which changes as the fielder runs) that is directly in front of the fielder's nose. The angle γ is, therefore, not the same as the angle β. A diagram of the unified fielder theory is shown in Figure 1. The figure shows a theoretical fielder consisting of two independent control systems. One control system controls a perception, p v, of vertical optical velocity, q v, and the other controls a perception, p l, of lateral optical displacement, q l. The system controlling p v acts to keep this perception under control by moving forward or backward (represented by variations in the output variable, f v ), as necessary. The system controlling p l acts to keep this perception under control by moving left or right (represented by variations in f l ), as appropriate. Each control system has a different reference for the perception it controls. The reference for the system controlling p v, is r v. The value of r v was a constant, 0.08, a value that gave the best fit to the data. A reference of 0.08 means that the system controlling p v tries to keep α increasing at a constant rate (.08 radians/sec, in this case). The reference for the system controlling p l is r l. The value of r l was set to 0.0, which means that the system controlling p h is trying to keep the image of the ball aligned with the image of the point directly in front of the fielder's nose. Figure 1 Here The main disturbance to the variables controlled by each control system is the movement of the ball itself, d. However, the forward and backward movements produced by the system controlling a perception of q v also act as a disturbance to q l. Similarly, the left and right movements of the system that is controlling a perception of q l act as a disturbance to q v. So although the systems controlling each variable act independently of each other, the systems interact via their disturbing effects on the variables controlled by the other system. Paths of Action and Perception The behavior produced by the model fielder is shown in Figure 2. Figure 2a shows the running paths taken by the model fielder. These paths are similar to those observed for real 5

fielders catching fly balls (McBeath et al, 1996, Figure 3, p. 571; McLoed et al., 2001, Figure 2, p. 1349). As in the case of real fielders, some of the paths produced by the model fielder are straight, some are concave inward, and some are concave outward. The shape of the paths produced by the model fielder depends on the trajectory of the ball (d) as well as the parameters of the two control systems. The degree and direction of concavity of the paths can be changed by changing the relative gains of the systems controlling p v and p l. The shape of the paths also depends on each system's reference for the perception it controls. Figure 2 Here Figure 2b shows the optical trajectory of the ball in terms of temporal variations in the angles α and β. These trajectories are again very similar to those observed for real fielders (McBeath et al, 1996, Figure 4, p. 572; McLoed et al., 2001, Figure 6, p. 1354). As noted by McLoed et al., the optical trajectories are often curved, especially when the ball is hit well to the side of the fielder. It is important to note that the optical trajectories shown in Figure 2b are not those of the controlled variables (p v and p l ) themselves. Figure 2b shows optical trajectories as they were plotted by McBeath et al and McLoed et al, in terms of the angles α and β. These optical trajectories were created by a model that was actually controlling perceptions of α and γ. Conceptualizing Control McLoed et al. describe their own control theory-based fielder model that catches fly balls by controlling elevation of gaze (p. 1349). Nevertheless, they conclude that there is still no unified fielder theory because we do not yet know why fielders choose a particular path to the ball. This conclusion is based on a concept of control that does not derive from control theory. The concept is that of a fielder who controls by choosing a particular path to the ball. This control of action concept of control assumes that the fielder s controlling consists of control actions (the paths taken) that are guided by perceptions (such as elevation of gaze). Control theory takes into account the fact that the fielder s actions actually occur in a closed loop. The result is a concept of control as control of perception, where the fielder s controlling consists of actions that guide perceptions to reference states and protect them from disturbance. Both the control of action and control of perception concepts of control are evident in 57 descriptions of the fielder s control processes that were found in McLoad et al. The majority 6

of these descriptions (60%) explained the fielder s control process in terms of the control of perception concept of control. An example of a control of perception description of the fielder s control process is as follows: OAC theory suggests that [fielders] run so as to keep the rate of increase of tan (α) constant. (McLoed et al, p. on p. 1350). This statement reflects a concept of control as control of perception because it describes the control process as actions guiding perceptions: the fielder s action (running) guides the perception (rate of increase of tan (α)) to a reference state (constant). The remaining 40% of the descriptions of the fielder s control process explain this process in terms of the control of action concept of control. An example of a control of action description of the fielder s control process is as follows: OAC theory claims that the fielder decides which way to run by the way tan (α) changes with time. (McLoed et al, p. 1350). This statement reflects a concept of control as control of action because it describes the control process as perception guiding action: a perception (time changes in tan (α)) guides the fielder's action (the decision regarding which way to run). McLoed et al usually conceptualize the controlling done by fielders as the control of perception. Unfortunately, they also conceptualize this controlling as control of action. Apparently, McLoed et al see no conflict between these two ways of conceptualizing control and, thus, are able to look at control from these two perspectives simultaneously. The only problem with doing this is that it has led to the discovery of obstacles to a unified fielder theory that do not actually exist. McLoed et al were looking at the fielder s controlling from a control of action perspective when they discovered that optical trajectory offers no clue to the fielder about which way to run (p. 1350) and, thus, cannot be the basis for the fielder s path choice. If McLoed at al were able to consistently look at the fielder s controlling as control of perception they would have known that this finding is irrelevant. A unified fielder theory depends on identifying the perceptions the fielder chooses to control, not the perceptions that tell the fielder which path to choose. Conclusion Control theory provides the basis for a unified fielder theory. McLoed et al (2001) were hot on the trail of such a theory but lost the scent to a red herring: the idea that we need to know 7

the perceptual basis of the fielder s choice of a particular path to the ball. This red herring appeared as a result of conceptualizing the fielder s behavior in terms of control of action. A unified fielder theory, based on a conception of the fielder s behavior as control of perception, accounts for the paths taken by the fielder in terms of the perceptions the fielder chooses to control rather than the paths the fielders choose to take. 8

References Chapman, S. (1968). Catching a baseball. American Journal of Physics, 36, 868-870. Marken, R. S. (1997) The dancer and the dance: Methods in the study of living control systems, Psychological Methods, 2,436-446 Marken, R. S. (2001) Controlled variables: Psychology as the center fielder views it, American Journal of Psychology, 114(2), 259-282 McBeath, M.K, Shaffer, D. M. & Kaiser, M. K. (1996). On catching fly balls. Science, 273, 258-260. McLoed, P., Reed, N. and Dienes, Z. (2001) Towards a unified fielder theory: What we do not know about how people run to catch a ball. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 120, 135-987 Powers, W. T. (1973) Behavior: The control of perception. Aldine-DeGruyter Powers, W. T. (1978) Quantitative analysis of purposive systems: Some spadework at the foundations of experimental psychology, Psychological Review, 85, 417-43 Tresilian, J. R. (1995) Study of a servo-control strategy for projectile interception, The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 48A, 688-715 9

Figure Captions Figure 1. Unified fielder model based on control theory. Figure 2. a) Paths of model fielder to intercept fly balls hit at various angles relative to the fielder's starting position. b) Optical trajectories, in α, β space, as seen by the model fielder running to intercept different fly balls. 10

Figure 1 r v r l p v e v p l e l i v () i l () o v () o l () Model Fielder q v f v q l f l Environment Laws of Optics d 11

Figure 2 a b 12