Imperceptibly off-center goalkeepers influence penalty-kick direction in soccer

Similar documents
processed by the perception system (e.g., Milner & Goodale, 2008). Hence, the distinction

Correlation analysis between UK onshore and offshore wind speeds

Chapter 1. The penalty kick in soccer often constitutes a moment that decides between winning or

An urgent call for clarity regarding England Rugby's injury claims

Analysis of the keeper-dependent strategy in the soccer penalty kick

Anxiety and attentional control in football penalty kicks: A mechanistic account of performance failure under pressure

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE. Full terms and conditions of use:

A novel phase-aligned analysis on motion patterns of table tennis strokes

Distribution competence of a football clubs goalkeepers

Effect of sea surface temperature on monsoon rainfall in a coastal region of India

Loughborough University Institutional Repository

An analysis of the air-jet yarn-texturing process part 5: the eect of wetting the yarns

Visual Search Strategies of Soccer Players Executing a Power vs. Placement Penalty Kick

Visual Scan Patterns and Decision-Making Skills of Expert Assistant Referees in Offside Situations

Measurement of underwater noise arising from marine aggregate operations

The Dynamics of Decision Making in Penalty Kick Situations in Association

Professor Stephen Hawking s World Cup Study for Paddy Power

An Application of Signal Detection Theory for Understanding Driver Behavior at Highway-Rail Grade Crossings

195 engaging and releasing club. 479 only releasing club. 166 all sides. 8,025 only player 10,282

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE. Full terms and conditions of use:

Minnesota Dance Team Scoring. Created by the Minnesota Association of Dance Team Coaches 2016

Is Home-Field Advantage Driven by the Fans? Evidence from Across the Ocean. Anne Anders 1 John E. Walker Department of Economics Clemson University

Wind assessment network at North of Yucatan Peninsula

Football Intermediate Unit

Yu Luo. Mitacs-Accelerate Grant (IT11777) Title: Using commitment to reduce plastics waste in the marine environment

Twisting techniques used in freestyle aerial skiing

Journal of Sports Sciences. On winning the lottery : Psychological preparation for football penalty shootouts

Although only 15.0% of the transfers involved a fee, global spending on transfer fees 2 during this one month period was USD 1.28 billion.

STEPHEN KIMBROUGH DAMJAN ŠTRUS SHOOT TO THRILL

Using Big Data to Model Football. Edward Griffiths Hong Kong Jockey Club 29 April 2015

The Importance of the Penalty Kick in the Modern Game of Football. Running heading title - Importance of the Penalty Kick in Football

ADVANCED CUE UTILIZATION OF SOCCER GOALKEEPERS DURING PENALTY KICKS. Tod Hanvey

Evaluation of Living Streets' Walking Works Pathfinder Employers Scheme

U6 Division Game Rules

Revisiting the Hot Hand Theory with Free Throw Data in a Multivariate Framework

Efficacy of Static and Dynamic Distance Perception on Kumite Performance in Karate

What Hippocrates called Man's best medicine: walking is humanity's path to a better world

From Bombe stops to Enigma keys

Impact Points and Their Effect on Trajectory in Soccer

Colored Entrance Treatments for Rural Traffic Calming

Lab 11: Introduction to Linear Regression

A Developmental Approach. To The Soccer Learning Process

Analysis of goals and assists diversity in English Premier League

FIFPRO World Player s Union, Legal Legends in sport and the future of sports law

LECTUR 10 CHARACTERISTICS OF THE DRIVER, THE PEDESTRIAN, THE VEHICLE AND THE ROAD. One problem that faces traffic and transportation engineers is:

EUROPASS SUPPLEMENT TO THE DIPLOMA OF

Available online at ScienceDirect. Procedia Engineering 112 (2015 )

Science in Sport. 106 How fast is the wind speed? Read. Count/Tachometer Any EASYSENSE. Sensors: Loggers: Logging time: EasyLog

Saturation flow mathematical model based on multiple combinations of lane groups

A study of football footwear bending stiffness

Open Research Online The Open University s repository of research publications and other research outputs

Two-stage pulse tube refrigerator with step displacer as phase shifter

Priorities for enhanced side impact protection in regulation 95 compliant cars

Traceable calibration of automatic weighing instruments in dynamic operation

Physical Fitness For Futsal Referee Of Football Association In Thailand

Putting to a bigger hole: Golf performance relates to perceived size

空手のカウンター状況における予測動作の熟練差の検討

Twisting techniques used by competitive divers

Walking Perception by Walking Observers

Batter Up! Define rehabilitate.

Is lung capacity affected by smoking, sport, height or gender. Table of contents

Mini Soccer What Game Format and Development Model is Best?

STUDY BACKGROUND. Trends in NCAA Student-Athlete Gambling Behaviors and Attitudes. Executive Summary

Running head: BATTING AVERAGE AND APPARENT BALL SIZE. Jessica K. Witt* and Dennis R. Proffitt. University of Virginia

RUNNING HEAD: DECEPTION AND THE QUIET EYE. TITLE: Aiming to deceive: Examining the role of the quiet eye during deceptive aiming.

The Effect of Bicycle Color on Visibility

Antonio Jesús Calderón Guillén Francisco de Asís González García Alejandro Lobato Manzano Sergio Mata Antolín

Licensed Coaches Event The England DNA: In the Grassroots game

PERSON RECOGNITION FROM DYNAMIC EVENTS: THE KINEMATIC SPECIFICATION OF INDIVIDUAL IDENTITY IN WALKING STYLE

An examination of try scoring in rugby union: a review of international rugby statistics.

The Effects of Altitude on Soccer Match Outcomes

The control of twisting somersaults

Required clothing ventilation for different body regions in relation to local sweat rates

Analysis of recent swim performances at the 2013 FINA World Championship: Counsilman Center, Dept. Kinesiology, Indiana University

A REVIEW OF AGE ADJUSTMENT FOR MASTERS SWIMMERS

Skills and strategies for soccer

CBRE Seminar. 30 th March Creating Shareholder Value. Budgeting Operational Expenses Commercial

Comparison of Selected Psychological and Anthropometric Characteristics between Successful and Unsuccessful Volleyball Players

Boom for a load handling machine.

save percentages? (Name) (University)

Whose game is it? Football governance through the eyes of the supporters [Paper #2: What could be done to improve football governance?

STATIC AND DYNAMIC EVALUATION OF THE DRIVER SPEED PERCEPTION AND SELECTION PROCESS

Notes on the Laws of the Game

Compensating tendencies in penalty kick decisions of referees in professional football: Evidence from the German Bundesliga

Black/White Stripe Used for Sportswear

Europeans & the World Cup

Results of mathematical modelling the kinetics of gaseous exchange through small channels in micro dischargers

Hypothermia, the Diving Reflex, and Survival. Briana Martin. Biology 281 Professor McMillan April 17, XXXX

Opleiding Informatica

RESEARCH REPOSITORY.

251 engaging and releasing club. 625 only releasing club. 236 all sides. 9,757 only player. 12,604 transfers

Talent Identification in Professional Soccer Players According to Their Birth Date

Richard S. Marken The RAND Corporation

ESSENTIAL QUALITIES OF THE GOALKEEPER IN FUTSAL

International Journal of Technical Research and Applications e-issn: , Volume 4, Issue 3 (May-June, 2016), PP.

In situ examination of decisionmaking skills and gaze behaviour of basketball players

BLACK SPOTS MANAGEMENT - SLOVENIAN EXPERIENCE

Building the Playing Style Concepts

Urban planning and traffic safety at night

Transcription:

Loughborough University Institutional Repository Imperceptibly off-center goalkeepers influence penalty-kick direction in soccer This item was submitted to Loughborough University's Institutional Repository by the/an author. Citation: MASTERS, R., VAN DER KAMP, J. and JACKSON, R., 2007. Imperceptibly off-center goalkeepers influence penalty-kick direction in soccer. Psychological Science, 18 (3), pp.222-223 Additional Information: Posted by permission of SAGE Publications. Metadata Record: https://dspace.lboro.ac.uk/2134/22025 Version: Accepted version Publisher: SAGE ( c Association for Psychological Science) Rights: This work is made available according to the conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International (CC BY-NC-ND 4.0) licence. Full details of this licence are available at: https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/ Please cite the published version.

Masters, R. S. W., van der Kamp, J., & Jackson, R. C. (2007). Imperceptibly off-center goalkeepers influence penalty-kick direction in soccer. Psychological Science, 18, 222-223. Imperceptibly Off-Center Goalkeepers Influence Penalty-Kick Direction in Soccer Masters, R.S.W. 1, van der Kamp, J. 1.2 & Jackson, R.C. 1 1. Institute of Human Performance, The University of Hong Kong 2. Faculty of Human Movement Sciences, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam The penalty kick generates a variety of strong emotions in soccer (Carroll, Ebrahim, Tilling, Macleod & Smith, 2002), and places the goalkeeper at such a disadvantage that only approximately 18% of penalties are saved (Kropp & Trapp, 1999). We asked whether a goalkeeper can, by standing marginally left or right of goal center, bias a penalty taker implicitly to kick to the side with greater area, thus allowing the goalkeeper to dive in that direction to make a save. We show that the penalty taker is unlikely to be mindful that the goalkeeper is off center, but nevertheless can identify the side with greater area and will be more likely to direct the penalty kick to that side than to the other side. Observation of 200 clips of penalty kicks, including those in World Cups, African Nations Cups, European Championships, and Union of European Football Association (UEFA) Champions League matches, indicated that goalkeepers stood marginally left or right of goal center on 96% of occasions. The mean displacement of the goalkeeper was 9.95cm, resulting in a

difference of 2.9% between the areas to the left and right of the goalkeeper (i.e., 2.9% was the difference between the two areas expressed as a percentage of total area around the goalkeeper, ( Area/Area x ). There was no association between the position of the goalkeeper and the side to which he dived (94 out of 1 dives were to the side with smaller area), χ 2 (1, N = 1) = 0.06, which suggests that the displacement was not a purposeful strategy. Nevertheless, regardless of the many factors that contribute to kick direction, more kicks were directed to the side on which there was greater area (103 out of 174), χ 2 (1, N = 174) = 3.45, P rep =.5, φ = 0.141. In an experimental study, we asked 51 participants to view 300 slides on a 15.4-in. Samsung wide-screen XGA monitor, from a distance of approximately cm. In each slide, a filled block was positioned on the goal line at one of 14 displacements to the right or left of center (the block and goal were scaled to 3% of the normal size of the German goalkeeper, Oliver Kahn, and a soccer goal, respectively). Participants were asked to judge on which side of the block there was greater area and to indicate the extent of their confidence in the judgment on a scale from % (low confidence) to % (high confidence). At differences in area as small as 0.5% participants showed low confidence in their perceptual judgment, yet indicated the side with the greater area at above-chance levels, t() = 7., P rep >.999, d = 1.078 (see Fig. 1b). In a more real-world replication, we replaced the block with an image of Oliver Kahn (e.g., Fig. 1a), and asked 20 participants to kick a football from a penalty spot (distance to screen = 4.8 m) to the side with the greater area and also to indicate their confidence that they has selected the correct side. The stimuli were projected on to a screen and were scaled to 44% of real life size. At differences in area of 0.5% and greater, participants accuracy was at above-chance levels, t(19) = 2., P rep =.959, d = 0.4 (see Fig. 1c), but only when the differences were at least 3.0% did participants show significantly increased confidence in their perceptual judgments (P rep =.999, ds > 0.482). Our data are compatible with Weber s Law in that the smallest difference in area at which participants showed reliably above chance discrimination was constant (~ 0.5%) regardless of the

scaling of the stimuli (i.e., 3% vs. 44% of real life size). The ability to discriminate differences as small as 0.5% is consistent with the demonstrated ability to discriminate differences as small as 0.1 to 0.8% in visual line-bisection tasks (e.g., McCourt & Olafson, 1997; Porac, Searleman & Karagiannakis, 2006; Scarisbrick, Tweedy & Kuslansky 1987). Such tasks require judgments of line length that are comparable with the relative area judgments in the present studies. In a third study, we assessed whether penalty direction is biased even when the kicker is unaware of the goalkeeper s displacement. To this end, we presented stimuli identical to those of the second study (again scaled to 44% of real life size) and instructed participants (N = 32) to take a penalty kick only when Kahn was standing in the center of his goal. At differences in area ranging from 1.6 to 3.0% participants directed their penalty kicks to the side with the greater area at above-chance levels (P reps >.926, ds > 0.378) despite kicking only when they believed that the keeper was in the exact center of the goal (Fig. 1d). The number of kicks dropped considerably at percentages greater than 3.0%, which suggests that participants were mindful of the fact that the goalkeeper was displaced to the left or right of center. In summary, we have shown that it is feasible for a goalkeeper to influence perceptions of area and consequently the direction of penalty kicks by standing marginally to one side or another of the goal center; the goalkeeper can then strategically dive to the side with greater area. Extrapolation of our data indicates that the optimum displacement of the goalkeeper in real life is from 6 to 10 cm. The penalty taker is unlikely to be mindful of a displacement in this range, but is at least 10% more likely to direct the penalty kick to the side with greater area than to the side with smaller area. Carroll, D., Ebrahim, S., Tilling, K., Macleod, J. & Smith, G.D. (2002). Admissions for myocardial infarction and World Cup football: Database survey. British Journal of Medicine, 325, 1439-1442.

Kropp, M., & Trapp, A. (1999). 35 Jahre Bundesliga-Elfmeter. Kassel, Germany: Agon Sportverlag. McCourt, M.E. & Olafson, C. (1997). Cognitive and perceptual influences on visual line bisection: Psychophysical and chronometric analyses of pseudoneglect. Neuropsychologia, 35, 369-3. Porac, C., Searleman, A., & Karagiannakis, K. (2006). Pseudoneglect: Evidence for both perceptual and attentional factors. Brain and Cognition, 61, 305-311. Scarisbrick, D.J., Tweedy, J.R. & Kuslansky, G. (1987). Hand preference and performance effects on line bisection. Neuropsychologia, 25, 695-699. Fig. 1. Influence of the goalkeeper s position (displacement from center) on perception and direction of penalties in soccer. An example of a stimulus display used in Studies 2 and 3 is shown in (a); in this particular display, there is proportionally more area (2.2%) on the right side of the goalkeeper than on the left. The graphs in (b) and (c) show results from the first two studies: participants accuracy in choosing (Study 1) or directing a kick toward (Study 2) the side with greater area and participants mean confidence in their judgment, normalized to the difference between the lowest and highest levels of confidence. The graph in (d) presents results from the third study, in which participants kicked only if they believed the goalkeeper was in the center. The graph shows the percentage of trials on which participants took a kick (black squares). All results are graphed as a function of the difference between the areas on the left and right sides of the goalkeeper, expressed as a percentage of total area around the goalkeeper, Δarea/area x. Error bars indicate standard errors.

a b accuracy confidence Accuracy (%) Confidence (%) c Accuracy (%) 0.5 0.7 1.2 1.4 1.8 2.1 2.5 2.8 3.2 3.5 5.3 6.9 10.4 22.1 ΔArea/Area x (%) accuracy confidence c c Confidence (%) d Accuracy (%) 0.3 0.5 0.8 1.1 1.4 1.6 1.9 2.2 2.5 3.0 3.8 7.7 15.4 ΔArea/Area x (%) accuracy kicks 0.3 0.5 0.8 1.1 1.4 1.6 1.9 2.2 2.5 3.0 3.8 7.7 15.4 Δ Area/Area x (%) 30 20 10 0 Kicks (%)