Entrainment in ballroom dances: The influence of the pair in the synchronization with the music

Similar documents
Amateur Dancer A person for whom dancing is a hobby and who does not seek financial gain from the teaching or performing of dancing.

Artistic Couple Dance Department. Street & Pop Dance Department. Artistic Acrobatic Dance Department. Stage Dance Department

AMATEUR MEDAL TESTS WHAT ARE AMATEUR MEDAL TESTS? FORMAT PREPARATION

Official Invitation VISEGRAD HUMAN INTEGRA CUP Sunday Bratislava hotel HILTON, Slovakia

Analysis of Swing Movement in Ballroom Dancing

AMATEUR MEDAL TESTS. What are Amateur Medal Tests?

2018 Solo Dance Series Requirements

DMIA March Dance Pageant Sunday 25 th March 2018

National Dance Council of Canada Conseil National en Danse du Canada

DANC-DANCE (DANC) DANC-DANCE (DANC) 1

TENTATIVE SCHEDULE OF EVENTS

Artistic Couple Dance Department. Street & Pop Dance Department. Artistic Acrobatic Dance Department. Stage Dance Department

Welcome to the 2015 Jacksonville Star Ball

WORLD DANCE ARTS FOUNDATION. YOUTH Ballroom Blitz 2012 Registration

BALLROOM STYLE & LATIN STYLE COMPETITION OFFICIAL RULES AND REGULATIONS NOVEMBER 2018

REQUIREMENTS FOR CERTIFICATION

2016 DANCESPORT NEW ZEALAND CHAMPIONSHIPS 1 October 2016

CLOSING DATE FOR ENTRIES: 13 April 2018

The Society of Australian Teachers of Dancing Ltd

New Mexico Senior Olympics Dance Rules Revised

Solo Dance Series Handbook for Skaters & Coaches

WDSF PROFESSIONAL DIVISION COMPETITION RULES

Ballroom and Latin Dance Rhythms for Drums Basics and Beyond by Fred Bolder

Dancing To Contemporary Songs - How to Select The Dance That Best Fits The Music Playing.

2015 IPC WDS competition in Rheinsberg, Germany 02. and 03. May 2015

Let s go to Kahoot.it. Getting started Kahoot

DanceSport Spectacular

Tentative Schedule of Events

THE 58TH NEW ZEALAND OPEN ONE DANCE CHAMPIONSHIP

WISCONSIN STATE DANCESPORT CHAMPIONSHIPS PRO/AM & AM/AM SINGLE DANCE AND PROFICIENCY ENTRIES

the DanceSport Capital!

2018 Para Dance Sport Mainhatten Cup Germany 19 and 20 May 2018

Blackpool Dance Festival

Blackpool Dance Festival

Welcome to LaBlast What is LaBlast?

3. Receives remuneration (money or other benefits) as a prearranged fee for dance performances other than actual expenses (travel, room and board).

APPROACH RUN VELOCITIES OF FEMALE POLE VAULTERS

NATIONAL SMOOTH DANCERS, INCORPORATED (NSD) (A NON-PROFIT SOCIAL DANCE ORGANIZATION) NSD OFFICIALLY APPROVED DANCE RULES 2009

Solo & Duo Rules / Guidelines:

Saturday, February 3, 2018

Please Print Legibly. State: Zip: State: Zip:

Provincetown Dance Trophy The 5 th annual Boston Open DanceSport competition

The 2017 West of England Open Championships

INTERNATIONAL DANCE FEDERATION TECHNICAL RULES

ICE SKATING AUSTRALIA 2017 / 2018 Ice Dance Elements (30 th May 2017)

Martynas Kūra. Finland judging congress 2016

IPC WHEELCHAIR DANCE SPORT. IPC Wheelchair Dance Sport Rules and Regulations

Creation and Exploration of Musical Information Spaces

Beigang 2018 Para Dance Sport Open 4-8 October 2018 CHINESE TAIPEI

IDTA NATIONWIDE DANCES 2017 re-issued

DANCE PRE-EVENT TECHNICAL PACKAGE

The 2015 Morris Open Solo Ice Dance Competition

As a base for all Latin American Figures the technique and principles explained in Technique of Latin Dancing by Walter Laird are recommended.

2014 Solo Dance Series Complete Handbook

Wisconsin. state. WSDC April 25-29, Teachers. Students. Dancesport Championships. WOrld ClASS PfiSTEr HOTEl MilWAukEE, WiSCONSiN

Dances With Owls 2014

OFFICIALS Judges 10) Michel Guimond (Chair) 11) Kim Donaghy 12) Benoit Drolet 13) Delphine Romaire 16) Aleksandra Antonova

Martynas Kūra. Finland judging congress 2016

2017 Solo Dance Series Complete Handbook

5th Asian Indoor & Martial Arts Games Dance Sport Technical Factsheet

Making the Move: Transitioning your team from 6.0 to IJS

Guide to Line Dance Level Defi nitions

STAR COMPETITION TECHNICAL PROGRAM REQUIREMENTS (JUNE 13, 2017)

DVIDA International Latin Bronze Syllabus Figures Cha Cha Samba Rumba Paso Doble Jive 1. Basic in Place

I N T E R N A T I O N A L S K A T I N G U N I O N

MARCH MANIA. March 2-4, 2018 INNISFIL RECREATION COMPLEX Yonge St. Innisfil, ON. Sanctioned by Skate Ontario

Complex movement patterns of a bipedal walk

Peikang 2017 Para Dance Sport Open

2 3 LIVING ARTS CENTRE

Page 1 INTERNATIONAL BALLROOM SYLLABUS LIST

SAFSA Rules and Regulations E The discipline of Ice Dancing consists of:

Summary of the new USA Dance Syllabus Guidebook Used to Invigilate USA Dance Competitions

DANCE (DANCE) (DANCE)

Solo Series Reference Material Series

I N T E R N A T I O N A L S K A T I N G U N I O N

EDWARD G. PICKEN PRO-AM DANCE CLASSIC 2008 June 6-7, 2008

WORLD LATIN DANCE CUP TEXAS QUALIFIER MARCH 18TH, 2018 RULES

2017 Ohio Star Ball Collegiate Challenge Official Information Document

STUDIO: AGE/ EDAD ADULT 19+

Key Concept Culture. Related Concepts expression, structure. Global Context Personal and Cultural Expression

When these weight transfers are well-controlled, the hips take care of themselves.

RSA TRAINING GUIDE # 4 INTERNATIONAL TEAM AND SOLO DANCE COMMISSION

INTERNATIONAL SKATING UNION

INTERCOLLEGIATE FIGURE SKATING COMPETITIONS

Module 6.0 Ice Dancing

CANADIAN DANCESPORT FEDERATION

To: IPC Wheelchair Dance Sport. Bonn, 28 July IPC Wheelchair Dance Sport, Sport Forum. Dear NPC President/Secretary General,

LICENSED EVENT BY I.D.O. ΔΙΕΘΝΗΣ ΑΓΩΝΑΣ ΑΝΟΙΧΤΟΣ ΓΙΑ ΟΛΟΥΣ

The AEROBIC Newsletter Nr 26

VII. Press Dance International Dance Competition

INTERNATIONAL DANCE FEDERATION TECHNICAL RULES

The competition fee includes participation in all competition events. Rates are shown in the table below.

Dance Class: How To Waltz, Quick Step, Foxtrot, Tango, Samba, Salsa, Merengue, L By Paul Bottomer READ ONLINE

Commission Sportive Nationale de Danse sur Glace

FIGURES Code Group I Group Primary A Figures 100/102 1B LOIF-ROIF Change Eight

LADIES MEN LADIES PROGRAM REQUIREMENTS. 3 jump elements 7 jump elements. 3 spins 3 spins. 1 step sequence 1 step sequence 1 spiral sequence

CIDA Presents The Monash Dance and Dancesport Festival Sunday 10 th July 2016 Organisers: Don Mc Robert, Lorraine Perkins, Donna Bradley

Handbook. for New Competitors. Useful information on entering your first same-sex dance competition. Information Guidance. Tips.

FIGURES Code Regional Elimination ~ National Final Regional Final ~ National Elimination ROF-LOF Circle Eight. LIOF-RIOF Change Eight

TELFORD ICE SKATING CLUB

Transcription:

International Symposium on Performance Science ISBN 978-2-9601378-0-4 The Author 2013, Published by the AEC All rights reserved Entrainment in ballroom dances: The influence of the pair in the synchronization with the music Luís Xarez Faculty of Human Kinetics, Technical University of Lisbon, Portugal In personal interactions, such as ballroom dances, there is an element of competition tending to keep the rhythm of each element of the pair (maintenance tendency), as well as a component of cooperation (magnet effect) and attraction by the rhythm of the other. Not everyone has the same ability to synchronize their movements with timekeepers, such as musical beats. In this study we intended, in a first phase, to measure this ability to change the spontaneous motor tempo (SMT) to synchronize the movements of ten different styles of dance with the tempo imposed by a metronome (compulsory motor tempo; CMT). Then we intended to verify the influence of the other, in experienced pairs, in the synchronization of the CMT. The results indicated that male participants presented higher SMTs than the female members of the same pair and higher values in Latin styles relative to standard styles. For the central objective of the study it was found that couples tended to help one another positively when synchronizing with the CMT, with a decrease of 9% of individual mistakes and 18% of mistakes per couple. Keywords: entrainment; ballroom dances; spontaneous motor tempo; synchronization; interpersonal coordination To dance at a professional level requires several physical and psychological capacities. Among them are coordinative abilities (rhythm, balance, spatial orientation, etc.) and the capability to synchronize movements with timekeepers, such as musical beats. Ballroom dances are always performed in pairs, demanding interpersonal coordination capacity and the entrainment of two people to perform the same motor task. For many authors, such as Brown et al. (2006), dance movement generally mirrored the hierarchical arrangement of strong and weak beats found in musical rhythm patterns. But this

142 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG situation does not arise when dancers dance without music, being forced to find their own rhythm internally, in the absence of an external sound stimulus. Igor Stravinsky spoke of the need for choreography to realize a form independent of that of music, while still being measured against it (Craft 1934). From his work with choreographers he conceived the idea that movement was structured independently of music. There seems to be evidence that, regardless of musical structure, we all have a spontaneous motor tempo (SMT): a person s preferred rate of moving (walking, running, tapping, dancing); a comfort zone, expressing an individual tempo to perform motor tasks. The main objectives of the present study focused on the assessment of the capacity of each participant to get away from their SMT (measured without any sound) and to synchronize with a required metronome tempo (CTM), in a range between 95 and 155 bpm, executing the basic steps of each of the ten ballroom dances that are part of international competitions. Then we sought to determine the influence (positive, negative, or neutral) of the pair in the performance of that task. Participants METHOD The sample consisted of 12 (6 female, 6 male) competitive level ballroom dancers (professional and pre-professional). The average age was 25.92 years (SD=4.32), ranging between 21 and 33 years. The average dance experience was 9.17 (SD=3.99) years, with a minimum of 5 and maximum of 17. On average pairs danced together for 4.67 (SD=1.97) years. The criteria for sample selection were the experience of regular participation in international competitions, the practice of the ten styles of ballroom dances (standard and Latin), and a minimum of three years of work with the same pair. Materials In this study a digital metronome (EMT-888) was used to impose different tempos as well as a digital video camera (Panasonic Model No. NV-GS60E) to record the performances. Procedure Participants were asked to dance ten ballroom dances in front of a camera without any sound (SMT). After that, subjects were asked to follow the tempo imposed by the metronome for each dance (CMT), as solo and in pairs. Three observers (experts in dance) identified the SMT by watching the collected

INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 143 images and adjusting the metronome to match. In a second phase the observers analyzed the images taken with CMT to assess individual and paired performances and choose one of three possible conditions for each of the ten dances: synchronized, faster, or slower. RESULTS Regarding the average for the ten individual dances studied, the SMT ranged between 96.40 (SD=15.31) and 103.60 (SD=14.59) bpm, rumba being the slowest (M=79.41, SD=3.42) and jive the fastest (M=124.91, SD=5.46). Waltz, foxtrot, and tango showed low values (M=84.58, SD=3.94; M=84.75, SD=5.02; M=86.83, SD=4.70) and the remaining five dances were much higher: paso doble (M=101.66, SD=2.36), samba (M=105.50, SD=3.23), chacha-cha (M=105.66, SD=3.77), quickstep (M=106.50, SD=5.00), and Viennese waltz (M=107.75, SD=4.99), though still far from the jive values. Two clear tendencies were observed when comparing the two groups of dances and tempos of each subject in relation to its pair. The masculine partners always presented higher tempos than his female pair (see Table 1), with the average value of this difference equaling 3.71 bpm. All subjects showed higher values in Latin dances compared with the standard dances, retaining the previous intra-pair difference for all the cases. Table 1. Spontaneous Motor Tempos in bpm (SD), without metronome. 10 styles 5 Latin styles 5 standard styles M 1 99.10 (12.58) 99.40 (14.36) 98.80 (12.24) F 1 96.70 (13.70) 99.20 (14.89) 94.20 (13.61) M 2 100.00 (14.97) 104.00 (17.23) 96.00 (12.94) F 2 97.40 (14.18) 102.60 (15.37) 92.20 (12.19) M 3 100.30 (14.39) 106.40 (16.50) 94.20 (10.03) F 3 96.40 (15.31) 102.60 (18.06) 90.20 (10.26) M 4 103.60 (14.59) 107.20 (18.50) 100.00 (10.22) F 4 97.00 (16.32) 104.60 (16.83) 89.40 (13.11) M 5 100.40 (15.59) 106.60 (17.11) 94.20 (12.56) F 5 97.40 (14.71) 103.00 (16.05) 91.80 (12.30) M 6 100.30 (15.30) 104.60 (15.84) 96.00 (15.17) F 6 96.50 (15.28) 101.00 (16.36) 92.00 (14.40)

144 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG The values of CMT (with metronome) were higher (on average 25.75 bpm greater) than the ones presented for the SMT, but similar to those found in the world champions of recent years. Most of the participants (46%) had difficulty in getting out of their comfort zone, presenting an execution closer to the SMT than to the value requested in the CMT condition. In Table 2 we can see the results of the analysis carried out by experts for solo and pair situations. In general we can see that the presence of the other has reduced the mistakes, helping to fulfill the task of synchronizing with the CMT. With the help of the pair the individual mistakes decreased 9%, from 46% to 37% (from 55 to 44 in 120 possible). But this synchronization increase is even more marked when analyzing mistakes per pair, in which there was a decrease from 60% to 41% (from 36 to 25, in 60 possible). In the slower and faster dances there was a positive influence, while in two dances placed in the middle of the scale (tango=120 bpm, cha-cha-cha=125 bpm) the number of desynchronized pairs increased from 3 to 4. DISCUSSION We can say that the magnet effect overlapped the maintenance tendency, to use the concepts described by von Holst (1973) and Polemnia et al. (1995), which characterize the complexity of interactions. Coordination between the two individuals of the same pair will result from the combination of competition (maintenance of the preferred frequency of each one) and cooperation (attraction to the frequency of the other). In the present study the influence Table 2. Number of individual and couple mistakes in solo and dance pairs when synchronizing with the metronome. Metronome (bpm) Solo: Individual mistakes Solo: Mistakes per pair Pairs: Individual mistakes Pairs: Mistakes per pair Rumba 95 8 5 6 3 Waltz 100 1 1 0 0 Foxtrot 110 6 4 4 2 Paso doble 115 0 0 0 0 Tango 12o 5 3 8 4 Cha-cha-cha 125 5 3 6 4 Samba 135 4 3 1 1 Quickstep 140 11 6 8 4 Viennese waltz 150 9 6 8 5 Jive 155 6 5 3 2

INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 145 of the pair was mostly positive in the improvement of individual synchronization with an imposed external sound stimulus. But this effect is not linear since in two dances (tango and cha-cha-cha) a slight increase of errors was verified in the pair situation. Ballroom dances are mainly characterized by men who lead the women. By choosing pairs with at least 3 years of experience we tried to ensure that pairs were familiar and could interact with stability in the execution of the tasks. We found that all the male participants had a SMT above his pair, so to accomplish a task with a considerably fast tempo, the mutual support worked and the cooperation allowed the pair to carry more elements out of their comfort zone to successfully accomplish the required tempo. This cooperation strategy with faster elements looks natural and supports previous studies (Xarez 2011). Another interesting aspect relates to the type of motor tasks performed. In that sense it should be noted the total number of hits found in paso doble in both conditions, which may be justified by the rhythmic structure of this dance: it is more marked, binary, and similar to a march. Also, relatively good synchronization was demonstrated in the slow waltz, which had only one mistake in the solo situation that was corrected by the presence of the pair. On the other hand, in both the faster and slower dances the role of the pair has shown to be decisive for the reduction of errors and better synchronization to the imposed tempo. Address for correspondence Luís Xarez, Faculty of Human Kinetics, Technical University of Lisbon, Estrada da Costa, Cruz Quebrada 1495-688, Portugal; Email: lxarez@yahoo.com References Brown S., Martinez M. J., and Parsons L. M. (2006). The neural basis of human dance. Cerebral Cortex, 16, pp. 1157-1167. Holst E. von (1973). On the nature and order of the central nervous system. In R. Martin (ed.), The Collected Papers of Erich von Holst. Coral Gables, Florida, USA: University of Miami Press. Polemnia G., Amazeen I., Schmidt R. C. et al. (1995). Frequency detuning of the phase entrainment dynamics of visually coupled rhythmic movements. Biological Cybernetics, 72, pp. 511-518. Craft R. (1934). Stravinsky and Robert Craft: Memories and Commentaries. New York: Doubleday & Company, Inc. Xarez L. (2011). Dances of Cape Verde: Tempo, preferences and entrainment. In A. Williamon, D. Edwards, and L. Bartel (eds.), Proceedings of ISPS 2011 (pp. 209-214). Utrecht, The Netherlands: European Association of Conservatoires (AEC).