Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 190 ( 2015 ) nd GLOBAL CONFERENCE on PSYCHOLOGY RESEARCHES, 28-29, November 2014.

Similar documents
Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 116 ( 2014 )

Krav Maga Self Defense Training Guide

Krav Maga (Krav Maga, Self Defense) By Goerge Silva READ ONLINE

Krav Maga Self Defence & Fitness Training Free guide to beginning Krav Maga

KRAV MAGA GLOBAL GUIDE TO KRAV MAGA

An Analysis of the Components of Sport Imagery in Basketball Players

ABSTRACT OF THE DOCTORAL THESIS

Krav Maga (Krav Maga, Self Defense) By Goerge Silva READ ONLINE

To AJKA-International AJKA-I of PA Instructor Trainee s Report #31. Subject: Karate and Personality

Introduction Definition of decision-making: the capacity of the player to execute an action following some conscious tactical or strategical choice.

The Road to the Olympic Games: A Four-Year Psychological Preparation Program

DESIGNING PERCEPTUAL MOTOR SKILL DEVELOPMENT PROGRAMS FOR SQUASH

Investigation of Winning Factors of Miami Heat in NBA Playoff Season

Golf. By Matthew Cooke. Game Like Training

Chapter 7. Stress Inoculation Training

Sportsmanship Rating Scale in Tennis Competition with Young Players

Combat Acudo Explained.

A Case Study of Leadership in Women s Intercollegiate Softball. By: DIANE L. GILL and JEAN L. PERRY

ELITE PLAYERS PERCEPTION OF FOOTBALL PLAYING SURFACES

THE REFEREEING IN BASKETBALL- TRENDS AND OPTIMIZATION STRATEGIES OF THE TRAINING AND PERFORMANCE OF REFEREES IN A DIVISION

Establishment of Emergency Management System Based on the Theory of Risk Management

Krav Maga Worldwide Level Curriculum Guide READ ONLINE

Application and effectiveness of Taekwon-Do in the face of modern combat techniques and styles

Comparison of Three Tests for Assesing the Aerobic Aptitude to the Elite Swimmers

Age Group Organization

Football Development Unit

Available online at ScienceDirect. Procedia Engineering 112 (2015 )

Krav Maga For Law Enforcement By Mark Slane, SGT Brannon Hicks READ ONLINE

Benefits in effective scouting:

Easton Soccer Player Development Curriculum U16-18 Age Group (Grades 9-12)

The purpose of this study was to investigate college students on

Does wearing a wrist guard affect the site of wrist fracture in snow sports?

Combatives Plays Important Role at Fort Hood

Krav Maga gun defence tactics and techniques. By Eyal Yanilov and Jon Hegan

ENGLAND FENCING COACH EDUCATION SCHEME (2009) SABRE LEVELS 1 TO 4

Self-Defense Meets Fitness In Modern Martial Art

Statistical Model of the Wing Players who Participated in the Women s European Handball Championship, Serbia 2012

SURVIVAL TACTICS. Develops Skills for Defending Against Sudden Attacks Covers:

ScienceDirect. Quantitative and qualitative benchmarks in volleyball game at girls "cadets" level (15-16 years old) Doina Croitoru * ICSPEK 2013

Available online at ScienceDirect. Procedia Engineering 84 (2014 )

TAMPA BAY UNITED ACADEMY PHILOSOPHY & CURRICULUM

Article by Morgan Buchanan. Bac Fu Do (White Tiger Way) Kung Fu is a powerful composite style martial art taught by veteran Australian Kung Fu master

Subject: Introduction and Self-Defense proposal to your prestigious organization.

KSDS After School Enrichment

The Basis of The Self-Explaining Road

Available online at ScienceDirect. Procedia Engineering 112 (2015 ) 40 45

Efficacy of Static and Dynamic Distance Perception on Kumite Performance in Karate

ACHPERConference 2010 NET/WALL GAMES

OFFICER INVOLVED SHOOTING, Muskogee, Oklahoma, Jan. 17, 2015

SUMMARY OF SAFETY INVESTIGATION REPORT

ICSPEK Using the Overtaking Manoeuvre in High Performance Handball. Ioan Rudi Pris caru a*

Article by Tad Nelson

A Comparative Study of Running Agility, Jumping Ability and Throwing Ability among Cricket Players

ScienceDirect. Investigation of the aerodynamic characteristics of an aerofoil shaped fuselage UAV model

ScienceDirect. Long-distance, short-distance: triathlon. One name: two ways.

Basic Modern Army Combatives Program Strategy

UNITED STATES MARINE CORPS WEAPONS TRAINING BATTALION TRAINING COMMAND GARAND ROAD QUANTICO, VIRGINIA LESSON PLAN

STUDY OF PSYCHOMOTOR VARIABLES OF BASKETBALL PLAYERS AT DIFFERENT LEVELS OF COMPETITIONS

VISUOMOTOR CONTROL OF STRAIGHT AND BREAKING GOLF PUTTS 1

SURVIVAL TACTICS Develops Skills for Defending Against Sudden Attacks

HIGHER PE (National 6) TACTICS Pupil Workbook

YOUR 7 RULES FOR THE JUSTIFIED USE OF FORCE

KADOCHNIKOV SYSTEMA.

The Extreme SKILLZ Program

Building the Playing Style Concepts

An analysis on feasibility of park & cycle ride system in a Japanese local city

SITUATIONAL AWARENESS, DISORIENTATION and IMPROVING FIREFIGHTER SURVIVAL AT STRUCTURAL FIRES

COST (Dollars in Millions) FY07 FY08 FY09 FY10 FY11 FY12 FY13 Cost to Complete

The Kadochnikov Systema

Employ The Risk Management Process During Mission Planning

2ND KAPAP ACADEMY WARRIOR CAMP JULY 11-12, 2015!

Q1. Including the current year, how many years have you been on the team? % One Year % Two Years

England DNA at the Foundation Phase Age Phase Priorities

These guidance notes should be used together with the Climbing Wall Development Instructor handbook.

SOCCER TRAINING POTENTIONAL OR REAL INFORMATION

Teaching university students self-defense at gun-point: A laboratory study on chances of injuries

CANOE-KAYAK SPRINT INTENSIVE COACHING COURSE LEVEL 3 EXPERT COACH

Variation of Nordic Classic Ski Characteristics from Norwegian national team athletes

CANOE-KAYAK SPRINT INTENSIVE COACHING COURSE LEVEL 3 EXPERT COACH

Combat Feedback from US Army Combatives Instructors

ScienceDirect. Rebounding strategies in basketball

Available online at ScienceDirect. Procedia Engineering 112 (2015 )

Licensed Coaches Event The England DNA: In the Grassroots game


CIRCLE OF COMPOSITION. By Mathew Allan

steps to designing effective practice

Keywords: Highway Intersection, Intersection Accidents, Crash Type, Crash Contributing, Statistical Analysis, Design Factors

Getting over the advantage line : how to manipulate space.

The importance of t. Gordon Craig, Coerver Coaching Director

COMPARISON STUDY BETWEEN THE EFFICIENY OF THE START TECHNIQUES IN THE ROMANIAN COMPETITIVE SWIMMING

Study on Fire Plume in Large Spaces Using Ground Heating

SURVEY OF SYSTEM OF ATTACKS BY JUDOKA IN REGIONAL AND INTERREGIONAL MATCHES M. CALMET

Road safety training for professional drivers: worldwide practices

Running head: BATTING AVERAGE AND APPARENT BALL SIZE. Jessica K. Witt* and Dennis R. Proffitt. University of Virginia

Inter-analyst variability in swimming competition analysis

The KING S Medium Term Plan Physical Education Netball Programme

Available online at ScienceDirect. Jiří Zahálka*, Jiří Tůma, František Bradáč

Football Intermediate Unit

Methods of Improving College Students' Tennis Techniques in Tennis Teaching

4v4 Skills League. Introduction

Transcription:

Available online at www.sciencedirect.com ScienceDirect Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 190 ( 2015 ) 409 415 2nd GLOBAL CONFERENCE on PSYCHOLOGY RESEARCHES, 28-29, November 2014 Does practicing close combat improve the perceived ability to perform better? Ole Boe a * a Department of Military Leadeship and Tactics, Norwegian Military Academy, P.O. Box 42 Linderud, 0517 Oslo, Norway Abstract Problem statement: In modern military operations, soldiers and officers often face the risk of engaging the enemy in close combat or hand-to-hand combat. Since this type of fighting is considered very stressful, it is of importance to find a suitable and easy-tolearn hand-to-hand combat system that will enhance the soldiers and officers ability to function under stress. Purpose of study: The present study investigates whether participating in a one-day hand-to-hand combat course in the Israeli hand-to-hand system Krav Maga would lead to a better perception and understanding of how to perform more efficiently under stressful situations. Method: Norwegian military officers (n=43) completed a questionnaire with questions related to hand-to-hand combat training. Participants were also asked about their knowledge about physical and mental reactions under stress. They were given the same questionnaire before and after taking part in the hand-to-hand combat course. Findings and results: Participants felt that the hand-to-hand combat training they had received better filled their need for functioning as effectively as possible in stressful situations, compared to before the course. Participants also indicated that they felt that their knowledge of hand-to-hand combat skills was better after having completed the course. Furthermore, the participants were found to have a stronger need to practice hand-to-hand combat training on a regular basis after the course compared to before the course. Participants also indicated that they felt that their level of hand-to-hand fighting skills was better after having completed the course. Furthermore, participants did not feel that it was more necessary to have knowledge about how people react physically and mentally in situations with stress after the course. However, participants felt that they had acquired more knowledge about how people react physically and mentally in stressful situations after the end of the course. Conclusions: The results from the present study suggest that Krav Maga is a hand-to-hand combat system that quickly installs confidence in those who practice it. 2015 The Authors. Published by by Elsevier Elsevier Ltd. Ltd. This is an open access article under the CC BY-NC-ND license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/). Peer-review under responsibility of Academic World Research and Education Center. Peer-review under responsibility of Academic World Research and Education Center. Keywords: Close combat; Hand-to-hand combat; Krav Maga; self-efficacy; decision making; stress * Ole Boe. Tel.: +47-23099448. E-mail address: olboe@mil.no 1877-0428 2015 The Authors. Published by Elsevier Ltd. This is an open access article under the CC BY-NC-ND license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/). Peer-review under responsibility of Academic World Research and Education Center. doi:10.1016/j.sbspro.2015.05.018

410 Ole Boe / Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 190 ( 2015 ) 409 415 1. Introduction Being able to make decisions is a critical component of any human being s life skills. As a research field, decision making has attracted an increased attention since it is viewed as one of the most crucial phenomena for the understanding of how people behave (Payne, Bettman, & Johnson, 1992). For military officers, being able to handle stressful situations is a critical skill. Future wars will contain a large element of friction and this will demand a lot of the individual officer or soldier (Watts, 2004). The operational military environment today consists of several factors. Some of these factors are changes, uncertainty, complexity, and ambiguity. This creates a great need for soldiers and officers to be able to cope in various situations and conditions. It is logical to assume that individuals with a high self-efficacy will be more apt to believe that they can meet challenges although different stressors are present (Jex, Bliese, Buzzell, & Primeau, 2001). Bandura (1997, p. 3) has defined self-efficacy as [the] beliefs in one s capabilities to organize and execute the courses of action required to produce given attainments. In a study of a very physically demanding selection program in the US Special Forces it was found that self-efficacy had a significant impact on whether the soldiers completed the hard physical selection or not (Gruber, Kilcullen, & Iso- Ahola, 2009). Previous research has shown that certain psychological skills are critical for experts to be able to perform maximum in a variety of tasks and contexts (Ericsson & Kintsch, 1995; Ericsson & Smith, 1991; Janelle & Hillman, 2003; Williams & Ericsson, 2005). Being able to acquire and to interpret information is necessary in order to understand the environment we live in. Considering the nature of many military tasks, an officer will often have to cope with several decisions at the same time, often under severe amounts of stress. These types of situations are referred to as in extremis leadership (Kolditz, 2010). In addition, an officer engaged in hand-to-hand fighting will have to make decisions under extreme levels of stress. These decisions very often carry serious consequences, namely the possibility of being killed or wounded. Using your experience to make decisions in the field is a process referred to as naturalistic decision making (Zsambok, 1997). Typical naturalistic decision-making problems that military officers may encounter are illstructured problems. The environments in which the hand-to-hand fighting situations exist are uncertain and dynamic, with shifting, ill-defined, or competing goals. Furthermore, an officer s decision will be based upon feedback received often under time stress. There are often high stakes for the decision maker who is involved, for instance getting hurt or killed. For military purposes, it is of interest to investigate the efficiency of hand-to-hand combat training. It is also of interest to find out whether the officers think that the training given to them has any effect. Ultimately, the search for the most effective hand-to-hand fighting or close combat system is a never-ending process. Close combat is the military term for close range fighting with hand grenades and small arms. If you run out of ammunitions, or experience a weapon malfunction, you will have to continue with hand-to-hand combat or fighting (Försvarsmakten, 2001). You may still use your primary or secondary weapon to thrust and/or hit the opponent. In addition you will be able to use your hands or feet as they are part of the weapons of your body (Lichtenfeld & Yanilov, 2001). 1.1. Krav Maga Krav Maga is the official hand-to-hand combat system of the Israeli Defense Forces (IDF). It has also been adopted for civilian use by Imi Lichtenfeld in 1964 (Lichtenfeld & Yanilov, 2001). It has been further spread to civilians, military forces in other countries, and police and security units all over the world by the organization Krav Maga Global under the tutelage of Eyal Yanilov. Krav Maga means contact fight in Hebrew, and was created by Imi Lichtenfeld in 1948. It has since then been in use in the IDF and in various branches of the Israeli security services as a system for self-defense and hand-to-hand combat. Krav Maga has been constantly tested in real-life situations since the beginning of its introduction into the IDF (Lichtenfeld & Yanilov, 2001). Krav Maga is based on instinctive reactions, logical defense principles, and natural body movements. Under stress, complex and fine motor skills will deteriorate, and gross motor skills will be enhanced (Siddle, 1995). For a soldier to be effective in hand-to-hand combat, he should train on techniques that are based on gross motor skills.

Ole Boe / Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 190 ( 2015 ) 409 415 411 This will also enhance his possibilities to acquire a new technique as quickly as possible. Most techniques in Krav Maga consist of gross motor skills. The system is therefore easy to learn, and trainees can obtain a high level of proficiency within a short period of instruction. The techniques and the tactics used in Krav Maga work in uncontrolled, violent, and tough environments. This means that the system is suitable for military needs. Krav Maga is taught to practitioners of the system through a process known as accelerated learning. Accelerated learning and other similar learning processes have been subjected to exhaustive and comprehensive research (National Research Council, 1988, 1991). Accelerated learning is based on learning a new technique in small steps or in separate components. This enhances mastering the technique fluently, a process known as the hard-wiring process (Siddle, 1995). The result is that it then becomes easy to acquire new techniques. It is therefore suggested that this type of learning process enhances the ability to quickly learn hand-to-hand combat techniques (National Research Council, 1988, 1991). The purpose of the present study was to investigate whether participating in a one-day hand-to-hand combat course in the Israeli hand-to-hand system Krav Maga would lead to an improvement of the ability to perform better in stressful situations. 1.2. Hypotheses The following 7 hypotheses were postulated in the study. In hypothesis 1 it was expected that the participants would feel that the hand-to-hand combat training they had received would fill more of their need to function as effectively as possible in stressful situations compared to before the course. Hypothesis 2 stated that the participants would feel that their knowledge in hand-to-hand combat would have increased compared to before the course. In hypothesis 3 it was expected that the participants would feel a stronger need to practice hand-to-hand combat on a regular basis after the course compared to before the course. Hypothesis 4 further stated that the participants would feel that it was more necessary to have knowledge about how people react physically in situations with stress after the course compared to before the course. In hypothesis 5 it was further expected that the participants would feel that it was more necessary to have knowledge about how people react mentally in situations with stress after the course compared to before the course. Hypothesis 6 furthermore stated that the participants would feel that they had more knowledge about how people react physically in situations with stress after the course as compared to before the course. Finally, in hypothesis 7 it was finally expected that the participants would feel that they had more knowledge about how people react mentally in situations with stress after the course as compared to before the course. 2. Method 2.1. Participants Forty-three male officers from the Norwegian Army National Guard participated in the study. Participant ages ranged from 25 to 42 years (M=35.1). 2.2. Design The design was pre-post with seven of the questions in a questionnaire as the within-subjects factor. 2.3. Materials The questionnaire was developed specifically for the course by the author and consisted of 9 questions. Participants were first requested to indicate their gender, rank, how many years they had served in the army, and their age. They were then asked to indicate how many hours they exercised each week. They were given 4 different alternatives to choose between, and were asked to indicate which answer they felt was most applicable to them. They were then asked whether they had trained any kind of martial art or hand-to-hand combat before. Answer

412 Ole Boe / Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 190 ( 2015 ) 409 415 categories to this question were yes or no. If participants had trained any kind of martial art or hand-to-hand combat before, they were then requested to specify what style or hand-to-hand combat system they had trained, and for how long they had trained. They were only requested to answer these questions once before the course. The following five questions were intended to investigate the perception that the participants had regarding their unarmed close combat training. These questions were repeated in a second questionnaire. For these questions participants were given 4 different answer alternatives to choose from, and they were then requested to indicate their preferred choice. They were furthermore asked if they felt that there was a need for hand-to-hand combat training on a regular basis. Additional questions were asked to find out how the participants rated their present hand-to-hand combat skills. After this, they were presented with 2 questions to find out if the participants felt that it was necessary to have knowledge about how people react physically and mentally in stressful situations. Finally, participants were then asked about how much knowledge they felt that they had about how people react physically and mentally in stressful situations. The questions that participants were requested to ponder and then to indicate their answers for can be seen in Table 1. Table 1. Questions to be answered by the participants. 1. How often do you exercise? 1 2. Have you practiced any martial art or hand-to-hand combat before? 2 3. Do you feel that the hand-to-hand combat training you have had until now satisfies your need to function as effectively as possible in a stressed situation? 3 4. Do you feel a need for hand-to-hand combat training on a regular basis? 4 5. How do you rate your present hand-to-hand combat skills? 5 6. Do you feel that it is necessary to know how people react physically under pressure? 6 7. Do you feel that it is necessary to know how people react mentally under pressure? 6 8. Do you feel that you have knowledge about how people react physically under pressure? 7 9. Do you feel that you have knowledge about how people react mentally under pressure? 7 1 Scale ranging from less than 1 hour per week to more than 5 hours per week. 2 Yes or no. 3 Scale ranging from does not fulfil the need to fulfils the need. 4 Scale ranging from no need at all to a strong need. 5 Scale ranging from very poor to very good. 6 Scale ranging from not necessary to very necessary. 7 Scale ranging from no knowledge to a lot of knowledge. 2.4. Procedure The questionnaire was presented to the participants before any training had started. After having filled out the first questionnaire, the participants were then immediately given a lecture that lasted one hour. This lecture explained which psychological and physiological changes that take place in the brain and body in dangerous or threatening situations. After this lecture they took part in an eight-hour training course where they trained selfdefense and hand-to-hand combat principles and techniques from the Krav Maga system. Finally, they were given the questionnaire again and were asked to fill it out again. The participants were then debriefed and were then free to leave. All questions were then coded either as 1, 2, 3, or 4 dependent upon which of the 4 answers that had been indicated. The only exception was the question regarding whether the participants had practiced any martial art or hand-to-hand combat before. The answers to this question were coded as either 1 for yes or 0 for no. 3. Findings and Results Most of the participants indicated that they exercised 1-3 hours a week. Thirteen out of the 43 participants answered yes to the question Have you practiced any martial art or hand-to-hand combat before. Those participants that had answered in the affirmative had practiced martial arts or hand-to-hand combat training for 6 months, on average. With the purpose of investigating whether there were any differences between participant s answers to the questions regarding close combat skills, analyses of variance (ANOVAs) were conducted. As shown below in Table 2, several significant differences were yielded.

Ole Boe / Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 190 ( 2015 ) 409 415 413 Table 2. Answers given in the questionnaire before and after training, mean values (M) Before training Questions M M After training 1. How often do you exercise? 1.91-2. Have you practiced any martial art or hand-to-hand combat before? 0.28 - If yes, for how long? 0,64-3. Do you feel that the hand-to-hand combat training you have had until 1.41 2.19*** now fulfils your need to function as effectively as possible in a stressful situation? 3 4. Do you feel a need for hand-to-hand combat training on a regular basis? 2.21 2,51* 5. How do you rate your present hand-to-hand combat skills? 1.79 2.30** 6. Do you feel that it is necessary to know how people react 3.19 3.16 physically under pressure? 7. Do you feel that it is necessary to know how people react 3.28 3.18 mentally under pressure? 8. Do you feel that you have knowledge about how people react 2.00 2.37* physically under pressure? 9. Do you feel that you have knowledge about how people react 1.98 2.32* mentally under pressure? *p<.05, **p<.01, ***p<.001 As can be seen from table 2, several interesting significant results were obtained. Hypothesis 1 stated that the participants would feel that the hand-to-hand combat training they had received would fill more of their need to function more effectively as in stressful situations as compared to before the course. This hypothesis received support as the participants score before the hand-to-hand combat training was (M=1.41) and the score after the course was significantly higher (M=2.19), p<.001. This was probably a result of the participants increased feelings of competency in performing the different techniques. Participants indicated that they felt that their knowledge of hand-to-hand combat skills were better than before the course began. Hypothesis 2 thus received support as a significant increase was found after completing the course (M=2.30) as compared to before the course (M=1.79), p<.01. This is probably a direct result of how easy the techniques were to learn and perform, as the techniques are based on accelerated learning. In hypothesis 3 it was expected that the participants would feel a stronger need to practice hand-to-hand combat on a regular basis after the course than before the course. The participant s answers showed a significant increase towards a stronger need to practice hand-to-hand combat training on a regular basis after the course (M=2.51) than before the course (M=2.21), p<.05. An explanation for this could be that participants felt that it was easy to increase their competency in performing the different techniques. The result of the training was probably that participants experienced an increased need to train hand-to-hand combat on a more regular basis. Hypothesis 4 stated that the participants would feel that it was more necessary to have knowledge about how people react physically in situations with stress after the course than before the course. This hypothesis did not receive support, as a very small non-significant decrease in the need to have this knowledge was shown by the participants after completion of the course (M=3.19) as compared to before the beginning of the course (M=3.16). An explanation for this might be that the participants already before the course had a good understanding of how people react physically in stressful conditions. Regarding hypothesis 5 it was expected that the participants would feel that it was more necessary to have knowledge about how people react mentally in situations with stress after the course as compared to before the course. This hypothesis was not supported, as only a non-significant small decrease in the need to have this knowledge was again revealed between before (M=3.28) and after the course (M=3.18). It is therefore proposed that participants did not feel that it was more necessary to have knowledge about how people react physically and mentally in situations with stress. It was furthermore expected in hypothesis 6 that the participants would feel that they had more knowledge about how people react physically in situations with stress after the course as compared to before the course. This hypothesis received support, as a significant increase was found in the answers that were given to this question

414 Ole Boe / Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 190 ( 2015 ) 409 415 between before (M=2.00) and after the course (M=2.37), p<.05. The feeling of having acquired more knowledge was probably a result of the theoretical lesson and participants becoming self-aware about the different processes taking place in their brain and body while being under stress. Finally in hypothesis 7 it was expected that the participants would feel that they had more knowledge about how people react mentally in situations with stress after the course as compared to before the course. Again, a significant increase was yielded between the answers participants gave to this question between before (M=1.98) and after completing the course (M=2.32), p<.05. The feeling of having acquired more knowledge about how people react mentally under stress is probably again a result of the combination of theoretical and practical lessons during the course. 4. Conclusions The aim of this study was to investigate whether training the Israeli hand-to-hand combat system Krav Maga would have any effect upon the participant s knowledge and confidence in close combat. The results from the present study suggest that Krav Maga is a hand-to-hand combat system that quickly installs confidence in those who practice it. This is probably due to the fact that the principles and techniques are easy to learn and perform. Combining these easy-to-learn principles and techniques with the understanding of how people react physically and mentally under stress further quickly lead to a perceived increase in confidence under stress. It was found that a oneday course significantly increased participants confidence in their own hand-to-hand combat skills. It was furthermore revealed that participants felt that the training they had received fulfilled more of their need to function as effectively as possible in a stressed situation as compared to before the beginning of the course. Finally, it was shown that participants acquired more knowledge about how people react both physically and mentally. The result of this study is promising as the search for the most effective close combat system is a never-ending process. Maybe Krav Maga can be the solution to this demanding process. However, more research is needed before any further conclusions can be drawn. Acknowledgements This research work was supported by the Norwegian Military Academy. The views expressed in this article are those of the author and do not represent an official position by the Norwegian Army. The author wishes to thank senior lecturer Merete Ruud at the Norwegian Military Academy for valuable assistance with the language of this paper. References Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York: W. H. Freeman and Company. Ericsson, K. A., & Kintsch, W. (1995). Long-term working memory. Psychological Review, 102, 211-245. Ericsson, K. A., & Smith, J. (1991). Prospects and limits of the empirical study of expertise: An introduction. In K. A. Ericsson & J. Smith (Eds.), Towards a general theory of expertise: Prospects and limits (pp. 1-38). New York: Cambridge University Press. Försvarsmakten (Swedish Armed Forces). (2001). SoldF. Helsingborg, Sweden. Gruber, K. A., Kilcullen, R. N., & Iso-Ahola, S. E. (2009). Effects of psychosocial resources on elite soldiers completion of a demanding military selection program. Military Psychology, 21:427 444, 2009. Janelle, C. M., & Hillmann, C. H. (2003). Expert performance in sport: Current perspectives and critical issues. In K. A. Ericsson & J. Starkes (Eds.), Recent advances in research on sport expertise (pp. 19-48). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics. Jex, S. M., Bliese, P. D., Buzzell, S., & Primeau, J. (2001). The impact of self-efficacy on stressor-strain relations: Coping style as an exploratory mechanism. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86, 401-409. Kolditz, T. A. (2010). In extremis eadership: Leading as if your life depended on it. Jossey Bass: San Francisco, USA. Lichtenfeld, I., & Yanilov, E. (2001). How to defend yourself against armed assault. Dekel Publishing House, Tel Aviv, Israel. National Research Council, (1991). In the mind s eye. National Academy Press, USA. National Research Council. (1988). Enhancing human performance. National Academy Press, USA. Payne, J. W., Bettman, J. R., & Johnson, E. J. (1992). Behavioral decision research: A constructive processing perspective. Annual Review of Psychology, 43, 87-131. Siddle, B. K. (1995). Sharpening the warrior s edge. The psychology and science of training. PPCT Management Systems Inc. IL, USA

Ole Boe / Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 190 ( 2015 ) 409 415 415 Watts, B. D. (2004). Clausewitzian Friction and Future War. Revised edition. McNair paper. Institute for National Strategic Studies, National Defense University. Retrieved from http://www.clausewitz.com/readings/w atts -Friction3.pdf. Williams, A. M., & Ericsson, K. A. (2005). Perceptual-cognitive expertise in sport: Some considerations when applying the expert performance approach. Human Movement Science, 24(3), 283-307. Zambok, C. E. (1997). Naturalistic decision making: Where are we now? In G. Klein and C. E. Zambok (Eds.), Naturalistic decision making (pp. 3-16). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.