HOW DO ELDERLY PEDESTRIANS PERCEIVE HAZARDS IN THE STREET?

Similar documents
Centre for Transport Studies

Centre for Transport Studies

Pedestrian vision and collision avoidance behaviour: Investigation of the Information Process Space of pedestrians using an eye tracker

Shared Space Delineators

EVACUATION SIMULATION FOR DISABLED PEOPLE IN PASSENGER SHIP

LECTUR 10 CHARACTERISTICS OF THE DRIVER, THE PEDESTRIAN, THE VEHICLE AND THE ROAD. One problem that faces traffic and transportation engineers is:

Cyclists gaze behavior in urban space: an eye-tracking experiment on bicycle facilities.

DOI /HORIZONS.B P23 UDC : (497.11) PEDESTRIAN CROSSING BEHAVIOUR AT UNSIGNALIZED CROSSINGS 1

An Examination and Recommendation for Current Practices in Roundabout Lighting

Pedestrian crossings survey in Europe


Low Level Cycle Signals used as repeaters of the main traffic signals Appendices

The Application of Pedestrian Microscopic Simulation Technology in Researching the Influenced Realm around Urban Rail Transit Station

Open Research Online The Open University s repository of research publications and other research outputs

Road Safety. Inspections

Pedestrian Dynamics: Models of Pedestrian Behaviour

PEDESTRIAN ACCOMMODATIONS DPS 201 AT ROUNDABOUTS

On street parking manoeuvres and their effects on design

Cambridgeshire floating bus stops interaction analysis

Effect of environmental factors on how older pedestrians detect an upcoming step

Clothing for Improving Mine Worker Visibility

Legendre et al Appendices and Supplements, p. 1

TYPES OF CYCLING. Figure 1: Types of Cycling by Gender (Actual) Figure 2: Types of Cycling by Gender (%) 65% Chi-squared significance test results 65%

Figure 2: Principle of GPVS and ILIDS.

Safety Assessment of Installing Traffic Signals at High-Speed Expressway Intersections

Introduction to Transportation Engineering. Discussion of Stopping and Passing Distances

Goodyear Safety Research Project 2008 Presentation by Competitive Measure at the FEI Eventing Safety Forum. Presented by Tim Deans and Martin Herbert

Transport Research Laboratory Creating the future of transport

From footway design to the street environment: removing the barriers to walking.

Modelling Pedestrian Route Choice Behaviour and Its Application to the Planning of Underground Shopping Streets

A STUDY OF SIMULATION MODEL FOR PEDESTRIAN MOVEMENT WITH EVACUATION AND QUEUING

VI.A-E. Basic Attitude Instrument Flight

University of Guelph DRIVE lab

Evaluation of shared use of bicycles and pedestrians in Japan

Real Time Bicycle Simulation Study of Bicyclists Behaviors and their Implication on Safety

XII.A-D. Basic Attitude Instrument Flight

HUMAN (DRIVER) ERRORS

City of Vallejo Traffic Calming Toolbox

~ A Behavioral Response Study in 2007 &2008 (BRS 07/08) was conducted in the Bahamas to

Introduction to Transportation Engineering. Discussion of Stopping and Passing Distances

A Hare-Lynx Simulation Model

Streets for All : 9 Use of white lines

RAMP CROSSWALK TREATMENT FOR SAN DIEGO AIRPORT, TERMINAL ONE

UAV-based monitoring of pedestrian groups

Glossary. Access Ramp: A slope which allows a wheelchair to enter or exit a location.

EUROPEAN COMMISSION DG RTD. evader

Undertow - Zonation of Flow in Broken Wave Bores

Trial 3: Interactions Between Autonomous Vehicles and Pedestrians and Cyclists

SPIRIT III Radiometer Saturation Effect

AGE, SKILL, AND HAZARD PERCEPTION IN DRIVING

Chicane Schemes. Traffic Advisory Leaflet 12/97 December Introduction

Enhancing Day And Night Visibility For Cyclists

Title: 4-Way-Stop Wait-Time Prediction Group members (1): David Held

TRIAL EVALUATION OF WIDE, AUDIO-TACTILE, CENTRELINE CONFIGURATIONS ON THE NEWELL HIGHWAY

Surf Survey Summary Report

Traffic Parameter Methods for Surrogate Safety Comparative Study of Three Non-Intrusive Sensor Technologies

Drift indication for helicopter approach and landing

Technologie et art du mouvement

Pedestrian Safety at Roundabouts

Urban planning and traffic safety at night

Technical Note. Determining the surface tension of liquids by measurements on pendant drops

Scissor Mechanisms. Figure 1 Torero Cabin Service Truck. Scissor Mechanism was chassis mounted and lifted the cabin to service aircraft

New power in production logging

#19 MONITORING AND PREDICTING PEDESTRIAN BEHAVIOR USING TRAFFIC CAMERAS

Dick Bowdler Acoustic Consultant

SCIENTIFIC COMMITTEE SEVENTH REGULAR SESSION August 2011 Pohnpei, Federated States of Micronesia

COLLISION AVOIDANCE SYSTEM FOR BUSES, MANAGING PEDESTRIAN DETECTION AND ALERTS NEAR BUS STOPS

Walk to School Week 2013

Active Travel Towns Funding Scheme Project Proposal. Sligo. Sligo Local Authorities

ScienceDirect. Rebounding strategies in basketball

STATIC AND DYNAMIC EVALUATION OF THE DRIVER SPEED PERCEPTION AND SELECTION PROCESS

Pedestrian traffic flow operations on a platform: observations and comparison with simulation tool SimPed

Using SHRP 2 s NDS Video Data to Evaluate the Impact of Offset Left-Turn Lanes on Gap Acceptance Behavior Karin M. Bauer & Jessica M.

Citywide Sidewalk and Crosswalk Programs

A proposal for bicycle s accident prevention system using driving condition sensing technology. Masayuki HIRAYAMA

VISUOMOTOR CONTROL OF STRAIGHT AND BREAKING GOLF PUTTS 1

Robot motion by simultaneously wheel and leg propulsion

A Case Study of Leadership in Women s Intercollegiate Softball. By: DIANE L. GILL and JEAN L. PERRY

RURAL HIGHWAY SHOULDERS THAT ACCOMMODATE BICYCLE AND PEDESTRIAN USE (TxDOT Project ) June 7, Presented by: Karen Dixon, Ph.D., P.E.

At each type of conflict location, the risk is affected by certain parameters:

The Effect of Pavement Marking on Speed. Reduction in Exclusive Motorcycle Lane. in Malaysia

Cycle journeys on the Anderston-Argyle Street footbridge: a descriptive analysis. Karen McPherson. Glasgow Centre for Population Health

23 RD INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON BALLISTICS TARRAGONA, SPAIN APRIL 2007

Low Level Cycle Signals with an early release Appendices

TRAFFIC SIGNAL WARRANT STUDY

Road Safety Attitudes and Perceptions of Pedestrians in Europe

Studying Vault Board Behavior: A Preliminary Look

Undertow - Zonation of Flow in Broken Wave Bores

Designing for Pedestrian Safety

D1.2 REPORT ON MOTORCYCLISTS IMPACTS WITH ROAD INFRASTRUCTURE BASED OF AN INDEPTH INVESTIGATION OF MOTORCYCLE ACCIDENTS

Walking aids based on wearable/ubiquitous computing aiming at pedestrian s intelligent transport systems

Public Workshop #2. September 14, 2016

AIS data analysis for vessel behavior during strong currents and during encounters in the Botlek area in the Port of Rotterdam

CHAPTER 132. Roundhead Stability of Berm Breakwaters

SELECTION OF LIGHTING CLASSES. a Comparative Analysis Version

Leak Checking Large Vacuum Chambers

if all agents follow RSS s interpretation then there will be zero accidents.

Saturation Flow Rate, Start-Up Lost Time, and Capacity for Bicycles at Signalized Intersections

How many mirrors and cameras is a truck driver able to look at? - Direct vision matters, evidence from London

Digiquartz Water-Balanced Pressure Sensors for AUV, ROV, and other Moving Underwater Applications

Transcription:

HOW DO ELDERLY PEDESTRIANS PERCEIVE HAZARDS IN THE STREET? An initial investigation towards development of a pedestrian simulation that incorporates reaction of various pedestrians to environments Summary Fujiyama Taku, Childs Craig, Boampong Derrick, Tyler Nick, Centre for Transport Studies, University College London, London, UK Corresponding email address: taku.fujiyama@ucl.ac.uk We investigated effects of lighting on the fixation behaviour of an elderly pedestrian. The participant was asked on a platform in the PAMELA facility, which simulated street environments and various hazards under four different lighting conditions. The hazards included obstacles, steps and combinations of vertical and horizontal gaps. In low lighting conditions, the participant fixated hazard objects more than orientation objects. For ascending steps, the duration of fixations corresponded to the step height, whereas for descending steps, the duration did not largely change according to the step height and it was less than that for ascending steps. These results suggest that pedestrians pay attention to hazards according not to actual danger but to the visual perception. Regardless of actual danger, visually obscure objects do not attract the attention of pedestrians. Further investigation should be made on pedestrians perception of hazard and orientation objects as well as the illuminance level below which pedestrians need more time/duration of fixation to perceive hazards (threshold). Key words: fixation; street hazard; elderly pedestrian; lighting 1. Purpose of the study In order to evaluate the accessibility of street and transport environments, such as railway stations, we are now developing a pedestrian simulation that incorporates elderly and disable pedestrians and their interaction with various environments including hazards on the street. For this development, it is necessary to understand how elderly and disabled pedestrians perceive hazards in the street and transport environments. Many elderly people suffer from some visual impairment. A study in the UK suggested 12% of people aged 65 or over have binocular acuity of 6/18 or less (Van der Pols et al, 2000). It should be noted that a quarter of the UK population will be aged 65 or over by 2031 (The Government Actuary's Department, 2004). Because of age related changes of visual perception organs, elderly people suffer not only visual acuity problems but also other forms of visual disabilities, such as visual field loss and less contrast sensitivity. Lighting is considered to be an effective solution to let elderly and disable pedestrians perceive possible hazards in the street. Interestingly, British Standards for residential street lighting have not considered lighting needs of elderly pedestrians or pedestrians

with visual disabilities (e.g. Fujiyama et al, 2005). In order to design street lighting that incorporates elderly and visually disabled pedestrians, it would be useful to understand how lighting improves the perception of hazards by elderly and disable pedestrians. The aim of this paper is to understand how elderly pedestrians perceive different hazards and to address issues to be investigated in future research. This paper focuses on fixation patterns of elderly pedestrians on different hazards in the street under different lighting conditions. Analysing fixation patterns helps us understand how pedestrians perceive environments or hazards (Fujiyama, 2006). This paper presents the initial results of our analysis of the eye tracker data of an ordinary elderly participant. 2. Methods 2 1. Experiment overview The empirical work took place in the Pedestrian Accessibility and Movement Environment Laboratory (PAMELA) at University College London. PAMELA is a laboratory used to test existing and proposed pedestrian environments under controlled conditions. There are several elements of the laboratory, perhaps the most obvious being a computer controlled paved platform which can be varied in terms of layout, topography and surface type. The laboratory is also equipped with a lighting system. The lighting system is capable of representing lighting conditions from absolute darkness to near daylight, including various levels and colours of artificial lighting. The experiment was conducted as a part of our ongoing EMMA (Evaluating measures to enhance the mobility of older and disabled people) project. In this project, we recruit elderly people and people with various disabilities, and ask them to walk on the PAMELA platform with various street configurations, such as slopes, steps, crossfalls and obstacles. Their walking behaviour is then analysed and will be used as a reference dataset to assess the street environments. As an initial investigation of the EMMA project, this paper presents the experiment results of an ordinary elderly participant. The participant was a male aged 74. His visual acuity was 0 in LogMAR scale (equivalent to 6/6), and his contrast sensitivity was 1.65 by the Pelli Robson test. The participant walked on the PAMELA platform four times under different lighting conditions. The lighting conditions were 241; 27.0; 4.5; and 0.85 lux on average on the surface of the platform, and the lighting source used was high pressure sodium lamps, which are common for street lighting. The experiment order was 241; 27.0; 4.5; 0.85 lux. In order to analyse his fixation behaviour, we used an eye tracker. Details of the platform configuration and the eye tracker follow. 2 2. PAMELA platform configuration The platform consisted of 4 lanes: Baseline, Pamelina, Obstacle, and Step lanes. On Baseline lane, there was no hazard. Pamelina lane had a combination of small vertical

and horizontal gaps, which the participant had to step over. The size of the gaps was 55mm height and 300 600mm width. On Obstacle lane, we placed couples of 1.3m height posts on the lane like a slalom ski course, where the participant had to walk through the middle of each couple of posts. In Step lane, steps with various heights ranging from 125mm (to descend) to 125mm (to ascend) were situated. These lanes were adjacent to each other and the participant was told to walk on all the lanes at once under each lighting condition. The length of each lane was 10.8m. The participant took around 2.0 minutes to complete. (For the precise duration, please see total values in Fig 4.) Step Notes Steps (to ascend) Obstacle Pamelina Baseline Steps (to descend) Posts Pamelina gaps Starting point Movement of the participant 10.8m Fig 1. Platform configuration 2 3. Eye tracker An iviewx Head Mounted Eye Tracking device (HED) for measuring eye movement (gaze position in this experiment) in response to visual stimulus, was used to analyse the fixation behaviour. The gaze position is the point in the person s field of view, where the eye is actually looking. The light weight helmet HED (manufactured by SMI) contains an Eye Camera (which records the eye movement), an angled Mirror (which directs the eye image to the eye camera) and a Scene Camera (which records the field of view of the person). HED also outputs the Scene Video with Gaze Position which is indicated by a Gaze Cursor in the field of view (see Fig 2). The video recording system in the computer can record the field of view every 0.04 second (25 frames per second) so that we know where the participant gazed each 0.04 second. In order to determine when the participant was fixating, we defined that, when the participant gazed at the same point in two or more video frames in sequence (if the participant continued gazing at the same point more than 0.04 second), the participant was regarded as fixating on the object. It was often observed that the participant slightly moved the point he gazed at. If the next gaze point was on the same object and within the range of 3.0 degree (roughly 10cm) from the axis to the original gaze position, the participant was regarded as gazing at the same point (and thus fixating). These numbers have been used in past research (e.g. Suzuki and Okazaki, 2002). Please note that,

fixation is a very slow eye movement corresponding to the person staring at a particular point, and contains small randomly drifting eye movements and quick adjustments to keep the target centred. 3. Results 3 1. Overall analysis Fig 2. An example of the field of view Recorded data were divided into 3 categories: Fixation, NG and No Fixation. Fixation indicates the data of fixation (seeing the same object in 2 or more frames) and the data can be analysed. NG shows the data in which the eye tracker cannot analyse where the participant gazed (e.g. the participant blinked). No Fixation means the data in which the participant did not continue gazing at the same point for more than 0.4 second and thus was regarded as not fixating. In Fig 3 and 4, Total sums up all the three categories. The data is presented as number, duration, and duration divided by number (average duration). Together, these parameters help us to characterise the participant s visual activity during the experiment. For example, the duration spent gazing at two objects can be similar, but if one object has a larger number of fixations than the other this could indicate a more complex route where the participant had to cope with more than one perceived obstacle. Fig 3 and 4 show the overall data summary. Percentage of the number of Fixation to Total is not so high in Fig 3, but the percentage of the duration is high in Fig 4. This implies that when walking the participant was always looking for gaze points, but actually gazed at a small number of points. Also, the figures show that there is not so much difference throughout the experiment in terms of the number and the duration of fixation. 3 2. Fixation patterns In this section, we look into Fixation data. Objects in the platform are categorised into Ground (PAMELA pavement surface); Obstacle (posts arranged on the platform for the participant to navigate around); Pamelina difference (the edge of PAMELINA gaps); Pamelina in between (spaces between edges of Pamelina gaps); Step (vertical

differences in the step section); and Target (marks on the edge of the platform showing 500 400 Number 300 200 Fixation NG: Cannot be analysed No Fixation: Too short Total 100 0 0.1 1 10 100 1000 Illuminance (lux) Fig 3. Data summary (Number) 150 Duration (Second) 100 50 Fixation NG: Cannot be analysed No Fixation: Too short Total 0 0.1 1 10 100 1000 Illuminance (lux) Fig 4. Data summary (Duration) the walking direction). Please also see Fig 1. Fig 5 shows the average fixation duration for all the categories. It was observed that Target shows higher values than other categories. The aim of the analysis here is to see

0.30 0.70 0.60 Average fixation duration (second/#) (except for Target) 0.20 0.10 0.50 0.40 0.30 0.20 Average fixation duration (second/#) for Target Ground Obstacle Pamelina Diff Pamelina inbet Step Target 0.10 0.00 0.00 0.1 1 10 100 1000 Illuminance (lux) Fig 5. Average fixation duration for all categories the effects of the illuminance on each category, and therefore in Fig 5 Target uses the right axis with the others the left. In Fig 5, Target, Ground, Pamelina in between, and Pamelina Difference show a U shape curve, whereas Obstacle indicates a steady decrease from the high illuminance toward the low illuminance. Fig 6 shows the total fixation duration for all the categories. It was observed that Ground shows higher values than other categories. The aim of the analysis here is to see the effects of the illuminance on each category, and therefore in Fig 6 Ground uses the right axis with the others the left. In Fig 6, Target and Obstacle show a positive proportionate relationship to the illuminance. On the other hand, Pamelina difference and Pamelina in between show a negative proportionate relationship. 3 3. Fixation patterns to steps In this section, we focus on Fixation on Step. It was observed that sometimes the participant gazed at an object situated not on the lane where he was walking but on another lane. In the analysis here, we extracted the data of Fixation on Steps made while the participant was on Step lane. Fig 7 shows the total fixation duration for the illuminance levels.

15 45 40 Fixation duration (second) except Ground 10 5 35 30 25 20 15 10 Fixation duration (second) for Ground Obstacle Pamelina Diff Pamelina inbet Step Target Ground 5 0 0 0.1 1 10 100 1000 Illuminance (lux) Fig 6. Fixation duration for all categories In Fig 7, a negative number of the step difference indicates the step the pedestrian descended, whereas a positive number of the step difference indicates the step the pedestrian ascended. If there was more than one step on the platform with the same step difference height, the fixation duration for that step height has been averaged over the number of steps. Fig 7 shows that, as the step height increases, the fixation duration increases. This increase is greater for positive step heights (ascending) than it is for negative step heights (descending). 4. Discussion This paper presents an initial result of our series of experiments aimed at understanding how elderly pedestrians perceive different hazards. It focuses on fixation patterns of elderly pedestrians on a variety of hazards in the street under different lighting conditions. There are three points to be addressed here. First, in Fig 5, some categories show a U shape curve. Why a U shape curve? In low lighting conditions, it can take time to get (visual) information about an object fixated because the object cannot be seen as clearly as in bright conditions. This can be one reason why average fixation durations are in a negative proportionate relation to the illuminance. On the other hand, given the experiment sequence (high illuminance to low),it is reasonable to assume that the participant did not need to fixate an object for a long time in the second run (27lux) and later (4.5 and 0.85 lux) because they already know it. This can be one reason why average fixation durations are in a positive

0.7 0.6 0.5 Fixation duration 0.4 0.3 0.85 4.5 27.0 241 0.2 0.1 0 150 100 50 0 50 100 150 Step Difference Fig 7. Fixation duration for steps proportionate relation to the illuminance. Because of these two reasons, the figure can show a U shape curve in some categories. It can be interesting to find out the illuminance level below which pedestrians need more fixation duration to perceive hazards because this level can be a threshold to assess street environments. In the presented research, average fixation durations to a hazard object show a U shape curve and the bottom of the curve is between 4.5 lux and 27 lux. More research is necessary to investigate this issue further. Secondly, in Fig 6, Target and Obstacle show a positive proportionate relationship to the illuminance, whereas Pamelina difference and Pamelina in between show a negative proportionate relationship. What does this mean? We could regard Target and Obstacle as orientation objects seen by the participant in order to decide the walking direction. Similarly, Pamelina difference and Pamelina can be regarded as hazard objects seen by the participant in order to detect possible hazards in front. Ground can also be interpreted as a hazard object because the participant fixated the ground in order to look for possible hazards. In low lighting conditions (or in later runs), the pedestrian fixated hazard objects more, whereas fixations on orientation objects decrease. This can be because in low lighting conditions, the participant may have been keen or tried to pay attention to hazard objects rather than to find out the correct walking direction. In addition, getting visual information of an object in low lighting conditions takes time. The decrease of the fixations on the orientation objects can also be due to the familiarity of the participant to the experiment site. Indeed, how pedestrian gather and cope with both hazard information of and

orientation information can be an important topic. When walking on a dark/misty road at night, we sometimes realise that we are in a wrong direction because we concentrated on finding possible hazards and missed the correct direction. On the other hand, when we walk in an unknown street, we tend to look at buildings along the street, which can be regarded as orientation objects (Chibana, 1999). Actually, in our experiments for different platform configurations, we observed that some participants did not recognise a step situated soon after the starting point and then tripped over it. This can be because the participants were concentrating on gathering orientation information. It would be worth investigating how pedestrians handle these two kinds of important information (orientation and hazard information) under sensory restricted spaces. Thirdly, Fig 7 shows that fixation on the steps with a positive step difference shows a positive proportionate relationship to the step differences. On the other hand, fixation durations to the steps with a negative step difference are in a negative proportionate relationship to the step differences. This leads us to infer that regardless of actual danger, visually obscure hazards do not attract the attention of pedestrians. In fact, the participant fixated 125mm gaps for less time compared with 125mm gap although a 125mm gap is also a potentially big hazard. Also, the fixation durations to 125mm gaps were not so different from those to 15mm or 10mm gaps even though a 125mm gap can be more dangerous than 15mm or 10mm. On the other hand, for steps with a positive difference, fixation durations to the step corresponded to actual danger (the more difference the more dangerous). These results imply that defects of the street design are not always perceived according to actual danger. As long as defects are inevitable, it can be not enough to make defects obvious. Making sure that pedestrians can recognise them is important. For example, it is impossible to demolish the gap between the train and the platform as long as a curve of tracks is situated in the station. The usual treatment for this hazard is to install a spot lighting or/and an audio alarm system. However, a question for this can be: Do all pedestrians, including the visually impaired, always notice it? If they do not perceive, they might trip. For these inevitable hazards, we should consider not only the presentation of the hazard and alerting systems (the design of facilities) but also perception of the hazard by pedestrians, including the visually impaired (the perception by pedestrians). It can be inappropriate to concentrate on design details without thinking about the perception by pedestrians. More research should be conducted on how pedestrians perceive hazards, and problems should be considered by a framework that incorporates not only the design of facilities but also the perception of hazards by pedestrians, including the visually impaired, and the interaction between design details and pedestrians perception. 5. Conclusion As an initial investigation of our series of experiments, the aim of this paper was, to look at the fixation behaviour of an ordinary participant. Although the results presented were only one ordinary participant s and thus the data size was too small to make an induction, the results suggested important and interesting points for the design of pedestrian

environments. The results suggested that the fixation behaviour on hazard objects and orientation objects are different, and that pedestrians pay attention to the hazards according not to actual danger but to the visual information obtained. Visually obscure hazards cannot attract attention regardless of actual danger. Further investigation should be made on the illuminance level below which pedestrians need more fixation duration to perceive hazards, on how pedestrians handle with both hazard and orientation objects and how attention to one kind affects the attention to the other, and on a framework that incorporates not only the design of facilities but also the perception of pedestrians and the interaction between design details and pedestrians perception. 6. Acknowledgement The authors wish to thank the UK Department for Transport for its funding for EMMA project, and Engineering and Physical Sciences Research Council (EPSRC) for its funding for PAMELA project. The authors wish to thank Mr. H. Rostron for various help and useful suggestions. 7. Reference (The web addresses cited were current on 31 Jan 2007) Chibana K, 1999, A study on the spatial abilities by eye fixation behavior, Journal of architecture, planning and environmental engineering, 520, pp159 164, in Japanese Fujiyama T, 2006, Investigating density effects on the awareness area of pedestrians using an eye tracker, CTS Working Paper 2006/4, Centre for Transport Studies, University College London, London, ISSN 1747 6232 Fujiyama T, Childs CR, Boampong DK, Tyler N, 2005, Investigation of lighting levels for pedestrians, Paper presented to WALK21 conference, Zurich, Switzerland, September 2005 SMI (SemsoMotoric Instruments). For more information, see http://www.smi.de/ Suzuki T, Okazaki S, 2002, Visual search while climbing or descending the staircases at a subway station, Journal of architecture, planning and environmental engineering, 558, pp151 158, in Japanese The Government Actuary's Department, http://www.gad.gov.uk/population/2004/uk/wuk04cc.xls Van der Pols JC, bates JC, McGraw JR, Thompson JR, Reacher M, Prentice A, Finch S, 2000, Visual acuity measurements in a national sample of British Elderly people, British Journal of Ophthalmology, 84 (2), pp 165 170