Game, set and match: The favorite-long shot bias in tennis betting exchanges.

Similar documents
What Causes the Favorite-Longshot Bias? Further Evidence from Tennis

Gamblers Favor Skewness, Not Risk: Further Evidence from United States Lottery Games

Staking plans in sports betting under unknown true probabilities of the event

Working Paper No

The sentiment bias in the market for tennis betting

Using Actual Betting Percentages to Analyze Sportsbook Behavior: The Canadian and Arena Football Leagues

An exploration of the origins of the favourite-longshot bias in horserace betting markets

Sportsbook pricing and the behavioral biases of bettors in the NHL

Behavior under Social Pressure: Empty Italian Stadiums and Referee Bias

Information effects in major league baseball betting markets

Original Article. Correlation analysis of a horse-betting portfolio: the international official horse show (CSIO) of Gijón

High Bets for the Win?

Peer Reviewed. Abstract

BIASES IN ASSESSMENTS OF PROBABILITIES: NEW EVIDENCE FROM GREYHOUND RACES. Dek Terrell* August Abstract

Hot Hand or Invisible Hand: Which Grips the Football Betting Market?

Inter-Market Arbitrage in Betting *

Individual Behavior and Beliefs in Parimutuel Betting Markets

Is Home-Field Advantage Driven by the Fans? Evidence from Across the Ocean. Anne Anders 1 John E. Walker Department of Economics Clemson University

Men in Black: The impact of new contracts on football referees performances

Market Efficiency and Behavioral Biases in the WNBA Betting Market

Racial Bias in the NBA: Implications in Betting Markets

Skewness is the name of the game

Are Longshots Only for Losers? A New Look at the Last Race Effect

Kobe University Repository : Kernel

An early warning system to predict house price bubbles

Revisiting the Hot Hand Theory with Free Throw Data in a Multivariate Framework

Are differences in ranks good predictors for Grand Slam tennis matches?

Going with your Gut: The (In)accuracy of Forecast Revisions in a Football Score Prediction Game

Framing Effects in the Field: Evidence from Two Million Bets

CS 5306 INFO 5306: Crowdsourcing and. Human Computation. Lecture 11. 9/28/17 Haym Hirsh

Extreme Shooters in the NBA

Decision-Making under the Gambler s Fallacy:

Power-law distribution in Japanese racetrack betting

International Journal of Computer Science in Sport. Ordinal versus nominal regression models and the problem of correctly predicting draws in soccer

Predicting Tennis Match Outcomes Through Classification Shuyang Fang CS074 - Dartmouth College

Explaining the Favorite-Longshot Bias: Is it Risk-Love, or Misperceptions?

Effects of Incentives: Evidence from Major League Baseball. Guy Stevens April 27, 2013

Review of A Detailed Investigation of Crash Risk Reduction Resulting from Red Light Cameras in Small Urban Areas by M. Burkey and K.

The Cold Facts About the "Hot Hand" in Basketball

Legendre et al Appendices and Supplements, p. 1

University of Groningen. Home advantage in professional tennis Koning, Ruud. Published in: Journal of Sports Sciences

Reference- Dependence in Dutch Football

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

JENA ECONOMIC RESEARCH PAPERS

a) List and define all assumptions for multiple OLS regression. These are all listed in section 6.5

Department of Economics Working Paper

CALIBRATION OF THE PLATOON DISPERSION MODEL BY CONSIDERING THE IMPACT OF THE PERCENTAGE OF BUSES AT SIGNALIZED INTERSECTIONS

Robust specification testing in regression: the FRESET test and autocorrelated disturbances

University of Nevada, Reno. The Effects of Changes in Major League Baseball Playoff Format: End of Season Attendance

Deciding When to Quit: Reference-Dependence over Slot Machine Outcomes

Contingent Valuation Methods

Swinburne Research Bank

Existence of Nash Equilibria

The Impacts of the Establishment of the Financial Holding Company on the Competition of Domestic Banking Industry

Gender Differences in Competition: Evidence from Swimming Data Shoko Yamane 1 Ryohei Hayashi 2

Strategic Forecasting in Rank-Order Tournaments: Evidence from Horse-Racing Tipsters

Fit to Be Tied: The Incentive Effects of Overtime Rules in Professional Hockey

In Search of Sure Things An Analysis of Market Efficiency in the NFL Betting Market

Tilburg University. Bets and Bids Potters, Jan; Wit, J. Publication date: Link to publication

Efficiency Wages in Major League Baseball Starting. Pitchers Greg Madonia

Explaining the Favorite Long Shot Bias: Is it Risk-Love or Misperceptions?

Measuring Match Importance in the Malaysian Super League Based on Win-Lose-Draw Results

Distancei = BrandAi + 2 BrandBi + 3 BrandCi + i

Peer Effect in Sports: A Case Study in Speed. Skating and Swimming

Factors Affecting the Probability of Arrests at an NFL Game

Overreaction in Football Wagers

Why are Gambling Markets Organized so Differently than Financial Markets? *

Journal of Quantitative Analysis in Sports

Determinants of college hockey attendance

Racetrack Betting: Do Bettors Understand the Odds?

Bookmaker and pari-mutuel betting: Is a (reverse) favourite-longshot bias built-in?

Are Workers Rewarded for Inconsistent Performance?

EFFICIENCY OF RACETRACK BETTING MARKETS. editors. Donald B Hausch. University of Wisconsin-Madison, USA. Victor SY Lo

Running head: DATA ANALYSIS AND INTERPRETATION 1

Title: Modeling Crossing Behavior of Drivers and Pedestrians at Uncontrolled Intersections and Mid-block Crossings

Rational Mispricing and Irrational Mispricing in Betting. Markets: Implications for Market Effi ciency Tests

Sub-Perfect Game: Profitable Biases of NBA Referees

1. Answer this student s question: Is a random sample of 5% of the students at my school large enough, or should I use 10%?

Honest Mirror: Quantitative Assessment of Player Performances in an ODI Cricket Match

Determinants of NFL Spread Pricing:

How To Beat The Bookie Scientific Betting In An Uncertain World By Rich Allen READ ONLINE

Professional Baseball: A Quantile Analysis. Wen-Jhan Jane* Department of Economics, Shih Hsin University, Taiwan

The Effects of Altitude on Soccer Match Outcomes

How Effective is Change of Pace Bowling in Cricket?

The final set in a tennis match: four years at Wimbledon 1

IS POINT SHAVING IN COLLEGE BASKETBALL WIDESPREAD?

5.1 Introduction. Learning Objectives

Should bonus points be included in the Six Nations Championship?

Traffic Parameter Methods for Surrogate Safety Comparative Study of Three Non-Intrusive Sensor Technologies

Human Capital Composition and Economic Growth at a Regional Level.

Equation 1: F spring = kx. Where F is the force of the spring, k is the spring constant and x is the displacement of the spring. Equation 2: F = mg

Measuring the Sorting and Incentive Effects of Tournament Prizes

Empirical Evidence on the Preferences of Racetrack Bettors

PREDICTING the outcomes of sporting events

Pierce 0. Measuring How NBA Players Were Paid in the Season Based on Previous Season Play

THE INTEGRATION OF THE SEA BREAM AND SEA BASS MARKET: EVIDENCE FROM GREECE AND SPAIN

Expectations as Reference Points: Field Evidence from Professional Soccer

Match Fixing and Sports Betting in Football: Empirical Evidence from the German Bundesliga

Stats 2002: Probabilities for Wins and Losses of Online Gambling

Predicting Exchange Rates Out of Sample: Can Economic Fundamentals Beat the Random Walk?

Transcription:

Game, set and match: The favorite-long shot bias in tennis betting exchanges. Isabel Abinzano, Luis Muga and Rafael Santamaria Public University of Navarre (SPAIN) June 2015 Abstract: We test for the existence of Favorite-Longshot Bias (FLB) in tennis betting exchanges. Despite these being order-driven markets, with no direct participation from bookmakers, we have found very similar results to those obtained by Lahvička (2014) for bookmakers betting markets: the bias is stronger in matches between lower ranked players, in later round matches and in high profile tournaments. This suggests that bookmakers adjustments to respond to informed betting are not the main driver of FLB. The varying magnitude of the bias across different types of event in the main market also weakens arguments linking FLB to gamblers risk preferences, and suggests the need to consider the microstructure features of the market together with the cognitive biases highlighted in the behavioral finance literature. Acknowledgements: This paper has received financial support from the Spanish Ministry of Economy and Competitiveness (ECO2012-35946-C02-01). Keywords: Favorite-longshot bias, sports betting exchanges, microstructure, market efficiency JEL Code: G02, G14, L83 Corresponding author: Luis Muga. Dpto Gestión de Empresas. Universidad Publica de Navarra. Campus de Arrosadia s/n. 31006 Pamplona (Spain). Phone: 34 948 166079. Fax: 34 948169404. e-mail: luis.muga@unavarra.es. 1

Game, set and match: The favorite-long shot bias in tennis betting exchanges. 1.- Introduction. The favorite-long shot bias is one of the most widely studied topics in the economic literature on sports betting markets. Basically, it can be explained as the phenomenon by which bettors obtain lower returns when betting systematically on long shots and higher returns when betting on favorites, and has been consistently reported for a wide range of sports and competition structures. Diverse arguments have been used to explain this phenomenon. It is attributed by Hurley and McDonough (1995) to the market presence of uninformed agents, who bet too much on unlikely outcomes and not enough on more likely ones. These authors find evidence to support their arguments in the horse racing betting market, where there are many potential winners and little is known about the participants. Others, such as Snowberg and Wolfers (2010) claim that it is due to the risk preferences of bettors, some of whom will be what the Cumulative Prospect Theory (Tversky and Kahneman, 1992) terms as local risk lovers. A third strand of theory (Lahvička, 2014) claims it to be the result of bookmakers altering the odds in response to informed betting, in the same way as market makers respond to informed trading by widening bid-ask spreads in price-driven financial markets (Easley and O Hara, 1987). The presence of heterogeneous beliefs among bettors may also generate FLB (see Ghandi and Serrano-Padial, 2015). Finally, various authors have proved that cognitive biases among bettors can skew market probability estimates, thereby altering the odds (Makropoulou and Markellos, 2011 or Abinzano et al., 2014). Recent evidence of the presence of FLB in traditional (bookmakers ) professional tennis betting markets appears in Forrest and McHale (2007) or Lahvička (2014). The latter argues that biased odds 1 are the result of bookmakers actions to back themselves against informed bettors. However, if this is in fact the explanation for the FLB phenomenon, it should not occur in betting exchanges where the odds are not set by bookmakers. We address this issue by testing for the presence of FLB in Tennis Betting Exchanges, where the odds are set by agents betting for and against a given outcome, as occurs in an order-driven market, thus, with no intervention by bookmakers. Past evidence shows that odds-setting efficiency is, in fact, higher in this type of betting market than in those where the odds are set by bookmakers (Franck et al. 2010 and 2013). It is important to note that evidence of the presence of FLB in the odds in a tennis match betting exchange would rule out explanations based on the dispersion of probability distributions over numerous outcomes (Hurley and McDonough, 1995) or the possibility of 1 The role of bookmakers in setting odds has also been documented by Kock and Shing (2008) and Rossi (2011). 2

it being mainly due to bookmakers odds-setting practices (Rossi, 2011; Kock and Shing, 2008; and Lahvička, 2014). Furthermore, the examination of the magnitude of the bias across different types of tournaments will enable us to corroborate, modify or deny the explanation linked to bettors risk preferences (Snowberg and Wolfers, 2010). If this were to be ruled out, the FLB might plausibly be linked to microstructure features, the degree of heterogeneity in agents beliefs or cognitive biases among bettors in these markets. 2.- Database The data, which were drawn from the betfair.com database, consist of bets placed in 28,595 professional tennis singles matches played between June 2004 and June 2013. The Betfair database includes both pre-event (PE) and in-play (IP) betting data for each event. We have not however used the odds on match outcomes once the event is underway (IP), because these would capture not only gamblers prior beliefs but also match result information. The database contains an entry for every odd taken in every market. For every match, we extracted or calculated the following variables: Event identification, Event description, Winner selection, Result (1, winner or 0, loser), Total bets, Total volume (pounds sterling), Average Bet (pounds sterling), number of different odds taken, odds standard deviation, value-weighted average odd, equal-weighted average odd, and high-volume odd. Also, following Lahvička (2014), we constructed dummy variables for whether the match was between players seeded below the top 50 in the professional ranking (LOWRANK), whether it took place after the first round (LATEROUND), and whether it was part of a major or grand slam event 2 (GS). Every bet place in this type of market involves one agent betting on the favorite and another betting on the longshot, therefore the random procedure model used by Lahvička (2014) is unnecessary in our case. It is important to note that for every match, the Betfair Company will, in addition, take bets on either the favorite or the longshot winning (or losing). Thus, there are two parallel markets: the main market where the bets are on the favorite and an alternative market where the bets are on the longshot. This alternative market proves particularly useful for testing whether markets with the same information, but with different types of gamblers, different expectations, or a varying degree of heterogeneity in agents beliefs, generate similar patterns of odds. 2 By major events, we mean the four Grand Slam (GS) events and the ATP and WTA final tournaments. 3

3.- Methodology and results. To test for the presence of FLB, we estimate the implied probabilities based on the odds in each market. In line with standard procedure in the literature, implied probability is estimated by inverting the odds (Forrest and McHale, 2007; Lahvička, 2014; and Abinzano et al., 2014). We define the AbRIP as the difference between the result of the bet and the implied probability. Under the null hypothesis of absence of FLB, the difference between the two variables (AbRIP) should be no different from 0 3. Table 1 presents the results for the variable constructed from the value-weighted average odds, equal-weighted average odds and high-volume odds, for the main and alternative markets. Table 1.FLB in professional tennis matches 2004-2013 VWODD EWODD HVOLODD MAIN Market AbRIP 0.0141** 0.0364** 0.0035 t-stat (5.67) (14.95) (1.43) ALTERNATIVE Market AbRIP -0.0428** -0.0396** -0.0507** t-stat (17.00) (16.00) (19.99) This table gives the test results of the variable AbRIP (Result Implied probability) for the different odds considered and the main and alternative markets. ** and * denote coefficients significant at the 5% and 10 % levels according to the t statistic adjusted by the White (1980) procedure. As can be seen, for all three odds estimates in the alternative market and for two of them (value- and equal-weighted average odds) in the main market, the evidence is consistent with the presence of FLB, allowing us to conclude that the basic explanation for this phenomenon does not lie in odds being set by bookmakers in other markets, as suggested by Lahvička (2014), since, in betting exchanges, there are no bookmakers to influence odds setting. In line with the same author, we then analyze the role played by bettors risk preferences in explaining FLB by measuring FLB levels in matches with different levels of uncertainty. For this, we run the following regression: = + + + + (1) 3 The result of the variable needs to be positive to indicate the presence of FLB in the main market, and negative to indicate it in the alternative market. 4

Table 2.FLB in professional tennis matches, 2004-2013, by event type MAIN VWODD EWODD HVOLODD α -0.0092 * 0.0084 * -0.0135 ** β 1 0.0348 ** 0.0366 ** 0.0185 * β 2 0.0111 * 0.0158 ** 0.0097 * β 3 0.0109 * 0.0164 ** 0.0165 ** ALTERNATIVE VWODD EWODD HVOLODD α -0.0163 ** -0.0152 ** -0.0298 ** β1-0.0511 ** -0.0539 ** -0.0424 ** β2-0.0144 ** -0.0078-0.0091 * β3-0.0003 0.0045 0.0043 This table gives the test results for equation (1) for the various odds estimates considered and for both the main and alternative markets. We use the OLS estimation method with White (1980) standard errors. **and * indicate levels of significance of 5% and 10%, respectively. Table 2 contains results for the main and the alternative market. The main finding is that the effect is significantly stronger for matches between lower-ranked players and for matches in later rounds of the tournament in both markets, and for matches in major tournaments in the main market only. Liquidity differences between the two markets (main and alternative) might explain this finding, but further examination is beyond the scope of our present objective. Another possibility, as suggested by Abinzano et al. (2014), is that the two markets attract gamblers of different types or different degrees of heterogeneity of beliefs. In any event, the confirmation of variation with the type of event in the main market weakens arguments linking FLB to gamblers risk preferences, which should not, in theory, vary by match type. Thus, although such explanations may hold some weight, their role cannot be decisive if, as shown, they lack the power to explain the results for tennis betting exchanges. 4.- Conclusions This study has turned up evidence of FLB in tennis betting exchanges. The presence of this bias in such a setting clearly rules out explanations based on the dispersion of probability distributions over numerous outcomes (Hurley and McDonough, 1995). 5

Meanwhile, the detection of this bias in markets uninfluenced by the direct participation of bookmakers shows that, in contradiction to the claims of Lahvička (2014), bookmaker intervention is not the essential condition for the FLB phenomenon. Furthermore, the result obtained for the main market, confirming that the effect is stronger in some types of event, enables us to downgrade explanations linking FLB to bettors risk preferences (Snowberg and Wolfers, 2010). The differences between the results for the main market (where the betting is on the favorite) and those for the alternative market (where the betting is on the longshot) suggest an interesting area for future analysis, which might reveal a mix of factors, including microstructure features, the type of gambler, or the degree of heterogeneity in agents beliefs in each market, acting in conjunction with the cognitive biases documented in the behavioral finance literature. REFERENCES Abinzano, I., Muga, L. and Santamaria, R. (2014) Behavioral Biases Never Walk Alone: An Empirical Analysis of the Effect of Overconfidence on Probabilities, Journal of Sports Economics, online first doi:10.1177/1527002514560575. Easley, D. and O Hara, M., (1987) Price, trade, size and information in securities markets Journal of Financial Economics, 19, 69 90. Franck, E.P., Verbeek, E., and Nüesch, S., (2010) Prediction Accuracy of Different Market Structures - Bookmakers Versus a Betting Exchange, International Journal of Forecasting, 26, 448-459. Franck, E.P., Verbeek, E., and Nüesch, S., (2013) Inter-market arbitrage in betting, Economica, 80, 300-325. Forrest, D. and McHale, I., (2007) Anyone for Tennis (betting)? The European Journal of Finance, 13, 751-768. Ghandi, A. and Serrano-Padial, R., (2015), Does belief heterogeneity explain asset prices: The case of Longshot bias. Review of Economic Studies, 82, 156 186 Hurley, W. and McDonough, L., (1995) A note on the Hayek Hypothesis and the favouritelongshot bias in pari-mutuel betting. American Economic Review, 85, 949 955. Kock, A.K., and Shing, H.F. (2008) Bookmaker and pari-mutuel betting: Is a (reverse) favourite-longshot bias built-in? The Journal of Prediction Markets, 2, 29-52. 6

Lahvička, J., (2014) What causes the favourite-longshot bias? Further evidence from tennis Applied Economic Letters, 21, 90-92. Makropoulou, V., and Markellos R.N. (2011) Optimal price setting in fixed-oddsbetting markets under information uncertainty. Scottish Journal of Political Economy, 58, 519 536. Rossi, M. (2011) Match rigging and the favorite longshot bias in the Italian Football betting market. International Journal of Sport Finance, 6, 317 334. Snowberg, E. and Wolfers, J., (2010) Explaining the Favorite-Long Shot Bias: Is it Risk- Love or Misperceptions? Journal of Political Economy, 118, 723-746. Tversky, A., and Kahneman, D. (1992) Advances in Prospect Theory: Cumulative Representation of Uncertainty. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 5, 297 323. White, H. (1980) A Heteroskedasticity-Consistent Covariance Matrix Estimator and a Direct Test for Heteroskedasticity. Econometrica, 48, 817-838. 7