Commentary. Managing risk from bears and other potentially lethal wildlife: predictability, accountability,

Similar documents
How people should respond when encountering a large carnivore: opinions of wildlife professionals

OREGON DEPARTMENT OF FISH AND WILDLIFE SUMMARY OF COUGAR MANAGEMENT IN NEIGHBORING STATES

Summary of discussion

Veronica Yovovich, Ph.D. Wildlife Conflict Specialist and Science Program Director Mountain Lion Foundation

Managing Encounters Between Humans and Coyotes. Guidelines and Information

Controlled Take (Special Status Game Mammal Chapter)

Livestock Losses. From Department of Agriculture report 2010 (report also available on this website)

Polar Bear Diversionary Feeding Workshop Report

MOUNTAIN CARIBOU INTERACTIONS WITH WOLVES AND MOOSE IN CENTRAL BRITISH COLUMBIA

Hundreds of encounters turn out okay because people react appropriately says DeBolt. Decades of education and bear advocacy by the WYGFD have saved

Black Bear Response Guide

COSEWIC Assessment and Status Report for Grizzly Bear Western population (Ursus arctos) in Canada SUMMARY

Frequently Asked Questions and Answers Regarding the Draft Northern Continental Divide Ecosystem (NCDE) Conservation Strategy

Stakeholder Activity

OREGON DEPARTMENT OF FISH AND WILDLIFE FIELD STAFF RESPONSE FOR COUGAR INFORMATION AND CONFLICT SITUATIONS

A SURVEY ON MOOSE MANAGEMENT IN CENTRAL ONTARIO

Competition. Competition. Competition. Competition. Competition. Competition. Competition. Long history in ecology

Comment Letter 1 for Item 5

Minnetonka Coyote Management. Hazing Works If We Do It Together

COYOTE MANAGEMENT PLAN. Purpose

Hatcheries: Role in Restoration and Enhancement of Salmon Populations

AOGA EDUCATIONAL SEMINAR. Endangered Species Act

EXECUTIVE SUMMARY Feasibility Study on the Reintroduction of Gray Wolves to the Olympic Peninsula

Grizzly Bear Conservation in Alberta: 2010 Management Activities and Recovery Implementation

AOGA Educational Seminar

Town of Mount Pleasant Coyote Management Plan

City of Isle of Palms, SC Coyote Management Plan

Beausoleil First Nation Eleven O-Gemaa Miikaan Christian Island, ON L9M 0A Fax:

PREDATOR CONTROL AND DEER MANAGEMENT: AN EAST TEXAS PERSPECTIVE

Challenges of Florida Panther Conservation. Presented by: Darrell Land, Florida Panther Team Leader

WildSafeBC Year End Report 2015 Kaslo and Area

MODULE 2. Conservation needs of cheetah and wild dogs and related threats to their survival. Notes:

Coyotes (Canis latrans) in Florida: the Good, the Bad, & the Ugly

Living With Your Wild(er) Neighbors. Kristin Cannon, District Wildlife Manager-Boulder North

A RESOLUTION OF THE CITY COUNCIL OF THE CITY OF CASTLE PINES NORTH APPROVING A COYOTE MANAGEMENT PLAN

Jeffrey Cole, Wildlife Manager Fish and Wildlife Dispatch

2016 West Coast Entanglement Summary

White-tailed Deer Management in Urban/Suburban Environments: Planning for Success

Living With Alligators. Florida Fish and Wildlife Conservation Commission

Mountain Caribou Recovery Implementation Plan Progress Board. Annual Report on Activities and Accomplishments of the Mountain Caribou Recovery

Town of New Castle. DRAFT Coyote Response Plan

Describe what is happening in figures 1-3. Is the population of mice different in figure 3 than in figure 1? Explain why.

Life history Food Distribution Management... 98

STATE OF MINNESOTA IN COURT OF APPEALS. Court File No. A Petitioners, Respondents.

Tyler Kuhn Yukon Department of Environment P.O. Box 2703 Whitehorse YT Y1A 2C6 Canada

REBOUND. on the. It was the winter of 2000/2001, and it seemed like the snow

Ministry of Forests, Lands and Natural Resource Operations

Early History, Prehistory

MOUNTAIN LION MANAGEMENT GUIDELINES FOR LION DAU L-1

Alberta Conservation Association 2016/17 Project Summary Report

Copyright 2018 by Jamie L. Sandberg

Application of a New Method for Monitoring Lake Trout Abundance in Yukon: Summer Profundal Index Netting (SPIN)

BETWEEN THE SPECIES Issue V August 2005

RE: Request for Audit of Ineligible Federal Aid Grants to Alaska Department of Fish & Game for Support of Predator Management

PETITION TO THE COURT

TWENTY-SIX YEARS. Delisting the Yellowstone Grizzly Bear. A Lesson in Cooperation, Conservation, and Monitoring

Canon Envirothon Wildlife Curriculum Guidelines

Polar Bear Interaction Plan

April Nisga a Fisheries & Wildlife Department

NEWS RELEASE. Harvest allocation ensures certainty for hunting sector

IN THE SUPERIOR COURT FOR THE STATE OF ALASKA THIRD JUDICAL DISTRICT AT ANCHORAGE ) ) ) ) ) ) ) ) ) ) ) ) ) ) ) ) ) )

H. R. To provide for the protection of the last remaining herd of wild and genetically pure American Buffalo. IN THE HOUSE OF REPRESENTATIVES A BILL

West District Bison Relocation Summary

Unit 1. Animals: Two Big Cats

Job Title: Game Management, Subsection B Game Management Mountain Lion

IMPROVING POPULATION MANAGEMENT AND HARVEST QUOTAS OF MOOSE IN RUSSIA

Invasive Species. Grade Levels. Introduction. This activity is intended for grades 9 12.

Mountain Goat Horns Of The Kootenay Region Of British Columbia

Invasive Versus Endemic Species

GRIZZLY BEAR MANAGEMENT CAPTURES, RELOCATIONS, AND REMOVALS IN NORTHWEST WYOMING

Endangered Animals at a Glance

Major threats, status. Major threats, status. Major threats, status. Major threats, status

FIVE YEAR SUMMARY OF EAST REGION WILDLIFE RESEARCH PROGRAM

RE: Development of an Environmental Assessment for a mountain lion management plan on the Kofa National Wildlife Refuge, Arizona

Subject: Scoping Comments Ochoco Summit OHV Trail Project

DRAFT G-1 DRAFT. United States Lifesaving Association Shark Bite Prevention and Response Guidelines January 6, p.1 of 4

Mountain Caribou Recovery Implementation Plan. Predator/Prey Component. Terms of Reference

Investigation of Regulating and Limiting Factors in the Delta Caribou Herd

STRATEGIES AND TECHNIQUES TO MINIMIZE WILDLIFE HAZARDS AVIATION. Matt Quick

ALBERTA WILDERNESS ASSOCIATION. Hunting, Trapping, and Fishing

Wolf Predation: Hunting Behavior and Predator- Prey Systems

Keywords: 7SI/Brown bear/harvest/harvest quota/hunting/malme/management/ mortality/population size/trend/ursus arctos

FINAL ENVIRONMENTAL IMPACT STATEMENT ON RESIDENT CANADA GOOSE MANAGEMENT Questions and Answers

Biodiversity and Conservation Biology

Top Cougar Biologist Weighs in on Oregon s Cougar Management Plan Posted by Predator Defense,

Matching respondents over time and assessing non-response bias. Respondents sometimes left age or sex blank (n=52 from 2001 or 2004 and n=39 from

Employ The Risk Management Process During Mission Planning

H. R To provide for the protection of the last remaining herd of wild and genetically pure American buffalo. IN THE HOUSE OF REPRESENTATIVES

Frequently Asked Questions Reintroduction of Bison to Banff National Park

Coexisting in Coyote Country

Describe what is happening in figures 1-3. Is the population of mice different in figure 3 than in figure 1? Explain why.

U.S. Fish and Wildlife Service Report

Life Transitions and Travel Behaviour Study. Job changes and home moves disrupt established commuting patterns

The 2001 Economic Benefits of Hunting, Fishing and Wildlife Watching in MISSOURI. Prepared by:

CHECKS AND BALANCES. OVERVIEW Students become managers of a herd of animals in a paper-pencil, discussionbased

National Report on Large Whale Entanglements

OREGON DEPARTMENT OF FISH AND WILDLIFE SUMMARY OF COUGAR POPULATION MODEL AND EFFECTS OF LETHAL CONTROL

YOUR 7 RULES FOR THE JUSTIFIED USE OF FORCE

Evolution by Natural Selection 1

Critical Incident Review Report

Transcription:

Human Wildlife Interactions 7(1):5 9, Spring 2013 Commentary Managing risk from bears and other potentially lethal wildlife: predictability, accountability, and liability Stephen F. Stringham, WildWatch LLC., 39200 Alma Avenue, Soldotna, AK 99669 USA wildwatch_consulting@yahoo.com Key words: bear, U.S. Forest Service, human wildlife conflicts, litigation, Ives, Ursus Accountability and liability of managers for protecting the public from wildlife should not assume an unrealistic ability to forecast attacks approaching the certainty of 20:20 hindsight after an attack. Although analysis of past attacks can yield valuable insights, it is likely to create exaggerated perceptions of average risk, of how much risk can be reduced by management actions, and of how predictable individual animals are during any given encounter. Such exaggeration can be minimized by understanding each species aggression within the whole scope of its social behavior, body language, and interactions with humans. Wildlife managers are responsible for keeping the public safe from wildlife, as well as for conserving wildlife species that, at least, occasionally injure or kill someone. Managers should balance the demands of public safety with those of public freedom to enjoy wildlands without killing animals unnecessarily. This is a balancing act that would work best if managers could make ballpark estimates of risk of attack under a wide range of scenarios, then tailor countermeasures to each scenario. Both estimating risks and countering them could be done in ways that assure accountability of managers who fail to act responsibly, yet, protect wise and diligent managers from unwarranted liability. The difficulties of this challenge are emphasized by the recent judgment against the U.S. Forest Service (USFS) for nearly $2 million. The award was made to the family of Samuel Ives, the 11-year-old boy who was camping with his family in Utah s Uintah National Forest, was killed by a black bear (Ursus americanus; Francis versus State of Utah 2010). At approximately 0500 hours on that same day, and at the very same campsite, the bear had ripped into another tent and struck another man in the head without injuring him. The victim and his friends drove the bear away, then, reported the incident to the Utah Division of Wildlife Resources (UDWR), which sent personnel with dogs to trail the bear and kill it. When the bear s trail was eventually lost several kilometers away and hours later, the trackers gave up for the day. Although an off-duty USFS law enforcement officer was notified of the morning incident, the report was not passed on to other authorities who might have assured that warnings were posted and that campers were verbally informed of the incident. Because of this oversight, the court found the USFS negligent, resulting in the boy s death (Kevan Francis and Rebecca Ives versus United States of America). A separate suit was initiated against UDWR (Francis versus State of Utah 2010). Without a fuller knowledge of those events and a deeper understanding of relevant law, I would not wish to question the court s decision. Nevertheless, I cannot ignore the wave of apprehension that this case is sending through wildlife-wildland management agencies. How will the Ives case affect future management of potentially lethal wildlife? Will agencies devote more of their dwindling resources to research on human wildlife conflicts to keep improving methods of protecting the public without shortchanging their other responsibilities? Or will sympathy for victims, sensitivity to bad press, and fear of exploitive litigation force agencies to focus on more direct means of defending themselves to forestall lawsuits or to win them in courts of law? One of the most counterproductive forms of

6 Human Wildlife Interactions 7(1) agency self-defense is excessively uniform and simplistic policy. Presumably, as long as there is official consensus within an agency or among agencies on which safety precautions to use, no one implementing them can be singled out for blame if and when they fail. Uniformity also facilitates educating agency personnel and the public about wildlife safety, because everyone is sending and receiving the same KISS (Keep- It-Simple-Stupid) messages. Nevertheless, any simplistic, 1-size-fits-all, set of precautions for a species is doomed to mediocrity. I offer the following 5 characteristics for a more sophisticated management policy. 1. It should be cost effective. Except in the case of emergency actions, any new precaution should be screened to assure that the gains in public safety outweigh any new constraints on public freedom in wildlands. For example, what would the costs and benefits to the public be of forbidding them to bicycle or walk dogs in a national forest or park frequented by bears? How would those policies affect wildlife? 2. A species, such as bear, should not be killed to increase public safety unless it poses a substantive risk and not merely pro forma because someone was frightened by the animal. Risk assessment should be based both on analysis of previous attacks and on knowledge about bear behavior that has not led to attacks. For example, managers should learn to distinguish offensive versus defensive threats and to recognize how likely any given behavior (e.g., jaw popping or pant huffing) is to presage a bear trying to injure either a human or a another bear (Stringham 2011). 3. Pressures for uniformity should not be allowed to stifle adaptation of management practices to situational variations in the ways that animals interact with humans. For example, agencies might continue to allow closely viewing of brown bears (Ursus arctos) and black bears on the Pacific coasts of Alaska and British Columbia where these animals are especially tolerant of one another and of people, but keep viewers farther from bears in habitats where the bruins are much less forgiving (Smith et al. 2005, Herrero et al. 2005). Viewers might be allowed greater flexibility when natural foods are abundant than when they are scarce and bears are less tolerant. Viewing might be encouraged only at sites where bears and people on foot can usually see one another from a distance of at least 100 m, but discouraged where bears and people frequently surprise one another at much closer distances. Habituation might be encouraged in situations where the benefits of reducing bear defensiveness the leading cause of brown bear inflicting serious or fatal injuries (Herrero 1985) outweigh any increase in offensive aggression (Stringham 2009, 2010). Professionals might be allowed to provide food to bears to lure them away from residential areas (Rogers 2011); but, all feeding of bears might continue to be forbidden where the reverse is true. 4. Policy should guide managers in making cost:benefit analyses. If numerous people are at risk from a bear, how much would their risk have to be reduced to compensate for any consequent increase in risk for a much smaller number of other people? For example, caribou (Rangifer tarandus) herds cross rivers each year, and numerous calves drown. Suppose the carcasses float downstream until they hang up on sandbars in an area frequented by anglers and brown bears are attracted to the carcasses. Recognizing high danger that some angler may be attacked by a carcassdefending bear, a proactive manager might decide that the carcasses must be moved and that the only feasible way is to float them downstream to areas seldom visited by people. Unfortunately, in the event that someone is mauled by a bear defending a relocated carcass, the likelihood that the relocation saved other people from injury might be offset by its elevating the risk to 100 percent for another individual. A similar problem might arise when managers haze a bear away from numerous anglers or from a residential area. The bear then may go toward another person, injuring him, a scenario the manager had not forseen. 5. Policy should assure that managers meet at least minimum standards of due diligence in each scenario that they are likely to face. Unlike attorneys who study past tragedies

Managing risk Stringham 7 to fix blame and liability, biologists study them to prevent future tragedies. Also, unlike attorneys, we biologists do not make our living by playing Monday-morningquarterback, focusing on a rare tragedy and dissecting it with the certainty of 20:20 hindsight. Hindsight concerning an attack might imply that once events A, B, and C occurred, tragedy was virtually inevitable. Indeed, if a bear has even once tried to prey on a human, most managers would expect more of the same and try to remove the bear. Some agencies might also do so even if a bear breaks into an occupied tent, as in the Ives case. However, if a bear s offense was merely foraging in campsites or defensively threatening someone who had stoned it, would this offense really increase risk of predatory attack by this bear? By how much? Merely knowing that factors A, B, and C might increase the likelihood of tragedy does not tell us how high that likelihood is (e.g., one in 10 2, 10 5, or 10 8 encounters). Nor does that knowledge alone enable us to predict the specific encounter during which A, B, and C will have tragic consequences, rather than the innocuous consequences they have had during countless other encounters. Precision and accuracy of prediction are limited by what is known about all the factors contributing to an encounter s outcome. The more managers know, the better they can forecast optimal actions for meeting their numerous competing priorities with limited resources. Identifying reliable risk predictors has been the goal of numerous studies on large carnivores, especially (a) bears (Herrero 1970, 1985; Herrero and Higgins 1999, 2003; Herrero et al. 2011; Miller and Tutterow 1998; Smith et al. 2005), (b) cougars (Deurbrouck and Miller 2001, Etling 2001, Mattson et al. 2011), and (c) wolves (McNay 2002, Graves 2007, Geist 2007). There also have been studies on ungulates (Geist 1978, 2011; Walther 1984). Popular accounts of bear attacks commonly are stated as simple, qualitative associations. Such an account, for example, might state that the majority of large North American mammals that kill or seriously injure people have at least one of the following traits: they are habituated, food conditioned, ill, injured, or malnourished; or, they are unskilled at foraging, capturing prey or coexisting with humans; or they have a history of prior aggression. So, too, a popular account might state that most victims have at least one of the following characteristics: they surprised the animal at close range; or, hey were associated with foods or other things that attracted the animal. Additionally, they might have been perceived by the animal as vulnerable; as resembling prey; as competing with the animal for space, territory, food or mates; or as threatening the animal or its offspring. Professional reports, on the other hand, are more likely to quantify associations, sighting the percentage of cases where the animal or the victim had one or more of those traits. This is illustrated by the findings of Herrero et al. (2011) that were based on all 63 instances of someone being killed by a black bear in North America since 1900: Of 56 fatal attacks by black bears, 88% were predatory; Of 36 fatal attacks, 92% were inflicted by an adult or subadult male; and 91% pf victims killed by black bears were alone or with just 1 other person. Based an such statistics, if someone were to be killed by a black bear, one might confidently predict that the culprit was a predatory adult or subadult male and that the victim was alone or with a single companion. However, such a conclusion would be made of course, after someone had been killed. It is not clear how such statistics can best help managers predict the likelihood of attack before one occurs. For example, what do Herrero s statistics tell us about risk of predatory attack in California or in any other state or province where no one has been killed by a black bear in the last 110+ years the same situation that existed in Utah until the Ives boy was killed? In fatality-free areas, are adult and subadult males really 11-fold more dangerous than females? And if so, how dangerous are females, e.g., 1 nonfatal attack per 10 3, 10 5, or 10 7 encounters with a human? Extrapolations from known killers to entire populations or metapopulations of a species are best treated as hypotheses in need of verification. The need for caution is perhaps best illustrated by recalling that most people

8 Human Wildlife Interactions 7(1) who kill other people are also adolescent or adult males. Yet, most human males never try to kill anyone. Case-by-case quantification of attack-risk by either bears or people requires information on a lot more variables than just age, sex, and species. For example, how much could risk of attack by an adult male black bear be altered (e.g., 0.1%, 50%, or 99.9%) by factors, such as shortage of wild foods, access to anthropogenic foods, conditioning of bears to those foods, habituation of bears to people, density of the bear population, or sport hunting of bears or ungulates? How much might a government agency reduce risk for visitors to a park by providing them with a bear-safety brochure, warning signs, or verbal instructions? How much can visitors reduce their own risk by following those precautions, e.g., wearing bear-bells while hiking or by surrounding tents with an electric fence? Statistics for attacks by bears would be most useful if they were directly comparable to statistics for more familiar animals, such as dogs, horses, and bees. The common practice of simply contrasting the numbers of people injured or killed by each species is highly misleading because it does not address differences in exposure (see Pritchard 2000). Exposure is partly a function of abundance of each species. When abundance is considered, the >5-fold higher annual lethal attack rate for all dogs over all bears (16 people killed by dogs versus 3 people killed by bears per year; Herrero 1985, Sacks et al. 2000) becomes a 19-fold higher rate per bear than per dog (1/220,000 bears versus 1/4.2 million dogs; Stringham, unpublished report). Even more insightful figures could be obtained if we could limit calculations to the numbers of bears and dogs that encounter a human and to the numbers of people who encounter a bear or a dog. Then, risks could be compared in terms of attacks per person per animal per encounter. Indeed, probability of being mauled during an encounter should be separated from probability of having an encounter, as Mattson et al. (2011) did for pumas (Puma concolor). This distinction is essential for evaluating risk factors (e.g., habituation), given that those which raise or lower probability of encounters may have a different effect on probability of attack during an encounter. Continent-wide summary statistics on risk levels and on factors that control risk level should eventually be integrated into a cumulative risk model that is designed so that managers could input knowledge about their local bear population. Then, such statistics should allow the model to compute both local baseline risk and how much risk could be modified above or below baseline by each environmental change (e.g., climatic warming) or management option. Without such information, how can managers assess the cost-effectiveness of each option so that their resources can be focused where they do most good to enhance public safety and to meet their other responsibilities, while minimizing unwarranted liability? How are managers, administrators, judges, juries or the general public to avoid either vastly overestimating or underestimating the benefits of each precaution that managers could implement? How are judges and juries to avoid overestimating the capacity of managers to prevent attacks without unacceptably constraining public enjoyment of wildlife and without preventively killing far too many benign animals? How are they to assess liability based on realistic understanding of the limitations of managers to predict and control what wildlife will do? Producing and implementing management plans with the 5 characteristics listed above would be markedly facilitated by better support for innovative research on wildlife behavior. If ursid predatory behavior had been better understood, then the quick return of the bear to the Ives campsite might have been predicted and adequate countermeasures taken. Knowledge can save both lives and money. Literature cited Deurbrouck. J., and D. Miller. 2001. Cat attacks: true stories and hard lessons from cougar country. Sasquatch, Seattle, Washington, USA. Etling, K. 2001. Cougar attacks: encounters of the worst kind. Lyons, Gilford, Connecticut, USA. Francis, K., and R. Ives versus State of Utah, Utah Division of Wildlife Resources. 2010. Findings of fact, conclusions of law and order, case No. 2:08CV244 DAK, <http://courtweb.pamd. uscourts.gov/courtwebsearch/utdc/ffss7txiih.pdf>. Accessed July 3, 2012.

Managing risk Stringham 9 Geist, V. 1978. Life strategies, human evolution, and environmental design. Springer, New York, New York, USA. Geist, V. 2007. An American wolf pack turns Russian. Pages 195 197 in W. N. Graves. Wolves in Russia: anxiety through the ages. Detselig, Calgary, Alberta, Canada. Graves, W. N. 2007. Wolves in Russia: anxiety through the ages. Detselig, Calgary, Alberta, Canada. Herrero, S. 1970. Human injury inflicted by grizzly bears. Science 170:593 598. Herrero, S. 1985. Bear attacks: their causes and avoidance. Lyons and Buford. New York, New York, USA. Herrero, S., and A. Higgins. 1999. Human injuries inflicted by bears in British Columbia 1960 1997. Ursus 11:209 218. Herrero, S., and A. Higgins. 2003. Human injuries inflicted by bears in Alberta1960 1998. Ursus 14:44 54. Herrero, S., A. Higgins, J. Cardoza, L. I. Hajduk, and T. Smith. 2011. Fatal attacks by American black bear on people: 1900 2009. Journal of Wildlife Management 75:596 603. Herrero, S., T. Smith, T. D. DeBruyn, K. Gunther, and C. Matt. 2005. From the field: brown bear habituation to people safety, risks and benefits. Wildlife Society Bulletin. 33:362 373. Mattson, D., K. Logan, and L. Sweanor. 2011. Factors governing risk of cougar attacks on humans. Human Wildlife Interactions 5:135 158. McNay, M. E. 2002. A case history of wolf human encounters in Alaska and Canada. Alaska Department of Fish and Game. Wildlife Technical Bulletin 13. Juneau, Alaska, USA. Middaugh, J. P. 1987. Human injury from bear attacks in Alaska, 1900 1985. Alaska Medicine 29:121 126. Miller, S., and V. L. Tutterow. 1998. Characteristics of non-sport mortalities to brown and black bears and human injuries from bears in Alaska. Ursus 1:239 252. Pritchard, P. 2000. Environmental risk management. Earthscan Publications, London, United Kingdom. Rogers, L. L. 2011. Does diversionary feeding create nuisance bears and jeopardize public safety? Human Wildlife Interactions 5:287 295. Sacks J. J., L. Sinclair, J. Gilchrist, G. C. Golab, and R. Lockwood. 2000. Breeds of dogs involved in fatal human attacks in the United States between 1979 and 1998. Journal of the American Veterinary Medical Association 217:836 840. Smith, T. S., S. Herrero, and T. D. DeBruyn. 2005. Alaska brown bears, humans, and habituation. Ursus 16:1 10. Stringham, S. F. 2009. Alaska magnum bear safety manual. WildWatch. Soldotna, Alaska, USA. Stringham, S. F. 2010. When bears whisper, do you listen? WildWatch. Soldotna, Alaska, USA. Stringham, S. F. 2011. Aggressive body language of bears and wildlife viewing: a response to Geist (2011). Human Wildlife interactions 5:177 191. Walther, F. R. 1984. Communication and expression in hoofed mammals. Indiana University Press, Bloomington, Indiana, USA. Stephen F. Stringham (pictured above) holds a B.S. degree in in biological oceanography from Humboldt State University, B.S. degree in wildlife management from the University of Alaska Fairbanks, and a Ph.D. degree in ecology from the University of Tennessee Knoxville. He spent several years studying ungulates, mother offspring relations in moose, and bear human interactions. He is the author of When Bears Whisper, Do You Listen? (Wildwatch, 2009). He is director of the Bear Viewing Association and owner of WildWatch, a wildlife consulting and research organization that also gives wildlife viewing tours and provides educational services.