LEARNING ABILITY OF ORPHAN FOALS, OF NORMAL FOALS AND OF THEIR MOTHERS

Similar documents
CHAP Summary 8 TER 155

Janne Winther Christensen

Bull Buyer s Guide. $3000 Purchase Price of New Bull Salvage Value of Old Bull (1900 lbs. X 1.10/lb.) $ 910 Net Cost of New Bull

Influence of Feeding Practices on Behavior and Activity Levels of Quarter Horse Mares

Effect of Maternal Dietary Yeast Supplementation. during Late Gestation and Early Lactation on Foal Growth. and Development from Birth to Weaning

Foal and Mare Behavior Changes during Repeated Human-Animal Interactions in the First Two Weeks after Foaling

4-H Horse Advancement Phase 3 Record Book

PERFORMANCE OF LIGHT WEIGHT STOCKER CALVES GRAZING SUMMER NATIVE RANGE WITH 25 OR 40% PROTEIN SUPPLEMENTS


THE ROLE OF THE SENSES IN MARE-FOAL

Value of Black Hereford Registration

Pearson Edexcel Level 3 GCE Psychology Advanced Subsidiary Paper 2: Biological Psychology and Learning Theories

Value of Black Hereford Registration

GENETICS OF RACING PERFORMANCE IN THE AMERICAN QUARTER HORSE: II. ADJUSTMENT FACTORS AND CONTEMPORARY GROUPS 1'2

Feeding the Broodmare

Day 1 Worksheet Foaling History and Due Date Calculation

0.5% chlorhexidine to apply to the end of the umbilical cord. Vet s telephone number to hand ( and inform vet that the foal is due)

Special Acknowledgement: J. Richard Trout, Ph.D. (biostatistics)

Metacognition: Metamemory in Horses

Progeny? Performance? What really matters in a stud fee. Charlotte R. Hansen and Sayed Saghaian. University of Kentucky

WHAT CAN WE LEARN FROM COMPETITION ANALYSIS AT THE 1999 PAN PACIFIC SWIMMING CHAMPIONSHIPS?

KALAMAZOO COUNTY 4-H HORSE PROJECT

CALF PERFORMANCE AND COW WEIGHT AND CONDITION FOR COWS SIRED BY HIGH AND LOW MILK EPD ANGUS AND POLLED HEREFORD BULLS

The effects of female cricket pheromones on the aggression of male Acheta domesticus crickets Nancy Gannon BIO 206L, Spring, , Ryan

COMMERCIAL BEEF SIRE SELECTION

Hair Shedding Scores Relating to Maternal Traits and Productivity in Beef Cattle. An Undergraduate Honors Thesis in the. Animal Science Department

Group Project. Horse Leader:

GENETICS OF RACING PERFORMANCE IN THE AMERICAN QUARTER HORSE: I. DESCRIPTION OF THE DATA 1'2

Salers 2015 Sire Summary Definitions

Dominance Hierarchies in Horses: Comparing and Contrasting Different Methods for Assessing Hierarchies

Sveriges lantbruksuniversitet Fakulteten för Veterinärmedicin och husdjursvetenskap Hippologenheten

BIOL 101L: Principles of Biology Laboratory

Economic Impact of the Michigan Equine Industry, 2006

2015 REPRODUCTION (easy) HIH910-5 Q: You do the math A mare that was bred on May 20th should foal in what month the next year?

Comparative Survival of Pellet-Reared Muskellunge Stocked As Fingerlings In Bluegill Ponds With and Without Largemouth Bass

An ocean-atmosphere index for ENSO and its relation to Indian monsoon rainfall

Hartmann s Mountain Zebra Updated: May 2, 2018

Effects of Creep Feeding on Preweaning and Postweaning Performance Of Angus x Hereford Calves

Chapter 5: Methods and Philosophy of Statistical Process Control

-SQA-SCOTTISH QUALIFICATIONS AUTHORITY. Hanover House 24 Douglas Street GLASGOW G2 7NG NATIONAL CERTIFICATE MODULE DESCRIPTOR

Contemporary Grouping for Beef Cattle Genetic Evaluation

IOWA 4-H LIVESTOCK JUDGING CONTEST

Equitation Science: A Research-based Approach to Improved Understanding of Horse Perspective

The UK Experience with use of Triploids for Restocking

Basic Data, Concerning the Rearing of an Orphan Foal

Status Report on the Yellowstone Bison Population, August 2016 Chris Geremia 1, Rick Wallen, and P.J. White August 17, 2016

ROUNDABOUT CAPACITY: THE UK EMPIRICAL METHODOLOGY

Bull management. Growing bulls 116. Selecting the best bulls to use 117. Managing working bulls 118. Assessing bull performance 120

To successfully work with horses, we need to understand their normal behaviour patterns and treat them as horses, not people, dogs or anything else.

The Effect of a Seven Week Exercise Program on Golf Swing Performance and Musculoskeletal Screening Scores

Care of the companion horse

CHAPTER ANALYSIS AND INTERPRETATION Average total number of collisions for a try to be scored

1. Answer this student s question: Is a random sample of 5% of the students at my school large enough, or should I use 10%?

Summitholm Holsteins Joe Loewith and Sons Ltd

The Rise in Infield Hits

Evolution by Natural Selection 1

Roundabout Design: Safety and Capacity Background Paper July 25, 2004

Ask the Vet April Stephen D. Fisch, DVM

Proceedings of the American Association of Equine Practitioners - Focus Meeting. First Year of Life Austin, Texas, USA 2008

The Irish Sport Horse Studbook Genetic Evaluation Report 2015

The Salmon Industry: Twenty-Five Predictions for the Future

Gait Analyser. Description of Walking Performance

Is lung capacity affected by smoking, sport, height or gender. Table of contents

Revisiting the Hot Hand Theory with Free Throw Data in a Multivariate Framework

Name of Member. Address County. Age as of Jan. 1, 20 Birth Date Grade in School. Club N ame No. Years Club Work. Organizational Leader.

Shoe-shaped Interface for Inducing a Walking Cycle

Conditioned Alarm Behavior in Fathead Minnows (Pimephales promelas) and Test Their Ability

NBA TEAM SYNERGY RESEARCH REPORT 1

THE AMERICAN QUARTER HORSE RACING JOURNAL

138 Trop Anim Prod :2

DEPARTMENT 6-J JUNIOR FAIR HORSE Entry Requirements: 1. Limited to exhibitors enrolled in the Horse (Equine) Project and the Horseless Horse Project.

Università degli Studi di Milano, Dipartimento di Medicina Veterinaria, Milano, Italy

ORGANISING TRAINING SESSIONS

8th Grade. Data.

Feeding the Broodmare.

A Hedonic Price Analysis of Risk Preferences in Yearling Thoroughbred Buyers. Xiurui Iris Cui

Housing types and paddocks

Introduction. Introduction. Introduction. The role of the mares. Model «copy of nature» Reproductive strategies: How to use nature to optimize results

STANDING STALLIONS TO THE PUBLIC

Exploring the relationship between Heart Rate (HR) and Ventilation Rate (R) in humans.

KENT EQUINE EDUCATION PROGRAM THE GIDDY UP PROJECT BOOK

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

Equine Rescue and Rehabilitation Ranch, Inc. Triple R P.O. Box 494 Tallahassee, FL 32302

Extreme Shooters in the NBA

NORMAL VS EARLY AND LATE WEANING: MANAGEMENT SYSTEMS TO ENHANCE PROFITABILITY DUE TO TIMING OF MARKETING

PASTURES FOR HORSES: CHALLENGES AND OPPORTUNITIES

What is going on in modern volleyball

HSIS. Association of Selected Intersection Factors With Red-Light-Running Crashes. State Databases Used SUMMARY REPORT

Basic Physical Needs of Horses

Application for diminishing or avoiding the unwanted. effects of traffic congestion

ACUTE TEMPERATURE TOLERANCE OF JUVENILE CHINOOK SALMON FROM THE MOKELUMNE RIVER

H FEEDER CALF PROJECT GUIDELINE

Animal Science Info Series: AS-B-226 The University of Tennessee Agricultural Extension Service

MATERNAL BEHAVIOUR OF MARES AND THE CONDITION OF FOALS AFTER PARTURITION

focus on behaviour Prevention is better than Cure

Grazing Strategies for Horse Owners

KENT EQUINE EDUCATION PROGRAM THE RIDER PROJECT BOOK

Relationships of Hip Height Measurements with Growth and Carcass Traits of Crossbred and Angus Cattle

The Handy Book of. Sigrid Schöpe HORSE TRICKS. Easy Training Methods for Great Results. Includes. of the World s Most Popular Tricks!

LENGTH OF THE WEANING PERIOD DOES NOT AFFECT POST-WEANING GROWTH OR HEALTH OF LIGHTWEIGHT SUMMER-WEANED BEEF CALVES

Transcription:

LEARNING ABILITY F RPHAN FALS, F NRMAL FALS AND F THEIR MTHERS Katherine A. Houpt I, Mary S. Parsons and Harold F. Hintz Cornell University, Ithaca, NY 14853 Summary The maze learning ability of six pony foals that had been weaned at birth was compared to that of six foals reared normally. The foals' learning ability was also compared to their mothers' learning ability at the same task; the correct turn in a single choice point maze. The maze learning test was conducted when the foals were 6 to 8 mo old and after the mothered foals had been weaned9 There was no significant difference between the ability of orphaned (weaned at birth) and mothered foals in their ability to learn to turn left (6 +.7 and 5.1 -+ 9 1 trials, respectively) or to learn the reversal, to turn right (6.7 -+.6 and 6.2 +.6 trials, respectively). The orphan foals spent significantly more time in the maze in their first exposure to it than the mothered foals (184 -+ 42 vs 55 -+ 15 s, Mann Whitney U = 7, P.05). The mothers of the foals (n = 11) learned to turn left as rapidly as the foals (5.9 +.7 trials), but they were slower to learn to turn right (9.8 + 1.4 vs 6.4 +-.4 trials, Mann Whimey U = 33, P.05), indicating that the younger horses learned more rapidly. There was no correlation between the trials to criteria of the mare and those of her foal, but there was a significant negative correlation between rank in trials to criteria and age (r = -.65, P.05) when data from the mare and foal trials were combined9 The dominance hierarchy of the mares was determined using a paired feeding test in which two horses competed for one bucket of feed. Although there was no correlation between rank in the hierarchy and maze learning ability, there was a correlation between body weight and rank in the hierarchy (r =.7, P.05). This may indicate either that heavier horses are likely to be dominant or that horses high in dominance gain more t Dept. of Physiol., New York State College of Vet. Med. weight9 Maternal deprivation did not appear to seriously retard learning of a simple maze by foals, although the orphans moved more slowly initially9 The lack of maternal influence on learning is also reflected in the lack of correlation between the mare's learning ability and that of her foal. Young homes appear to learn more rapidly than older horses9 (Key Words: Horse, Learning, Dominance, Pony, Weaning, Maze.) Introduction A foal sometimes must be reared artificially either because its mother has died or because the mare is unable or unwilling to properly nurse the foal. In addition to those instances in which the foal must be bottle reared, there are times when the owner may decide to wean at an early age in order to transport the mare to be rebred without subjecting the foal to the stress of travel or the danger of infectious disease on another farm. Because artificial rearing may have an effect on the foal's later behavior, we wished to determine if the lack of normal maternal care had any effect on the young horse's learning ability or potential to be trained9 The learning ability of adult (Gardner, 1937; Haag et al., 1980; Mader and Price, 1980) and juvenile (Kratzer et al., 1977; Fiske and Potter, 1979) horses has been studied, but the learning ability of artificially reared horses has not been compared with that of those normally reared9 Experiments with other species such as dogs (Melzack and Scott, 1957) and cats (Seitz, 1959) indicate that learning ability might be impaired9 Learning ability may alter a horse's trainability. If artificially reared homes learn more slowly, the information would be of considerable economic interest to the horse industry, because homes must be trained in order to be used for racing, driving or pleasure riding. 1027 JURNAL F ANIMAL SCIENCE, Vol. 55, No. 5, 1982

1028 HUPT ET AL. Materials and Methods Two groups of six grade Shetland-type pony,coals were used. The foals in one group, the orphans (n = 6), were taken from their mothers at birth and reared artificially. They were never allowed to suckle. The orphan foals were bottle fed for 48 h and then gradually shifted to a hay and grain diet. The second group, the mothered foals (n = 6), remained with their dams and were nursed by them. The details of the method of rearing the orphan foals have been given elsewhere (Burton et al., 1981). The maze learning experiments began when the foals were 6 to 8 mo old and 1 mo after the mothered foals were weaned. Both groups were fed a complete pelleted feed. The foals were weighed at 14 wk of age. Two of the orphan foals and three of the mothered foals were colts. The mother of one of the mothered foals died when the foal was 3 mo old. All the other mares (n = 11) were available for testing. They were weighed at the time of testing. Age was determined from inspection of their teeth. The design of the maze was similar to that described by Kratzer et al. (1977). The ponies had to choose between a right side and a left side escape, one of which could be blocked by a partition. A bucket of the pony's accustomed feed was placed approximately 1 m from the exit of the maze as a positive reinforcement. The ponies, both mares and foals, were fed morning (0800 h) and evening (1500 h). Two maze learning trials were conducted per day just before the evening feeding. Therefore, the ponies had been deprived of food for approximately 7 h. The order of testing was randomized, but those ponies that were performing a task different from the majority (i.e., turning left while others were turning right) were tested last. The study was divided into three phases. The first phase (trials 1 to 4 on the first 2 d) was to observe side preference. For the first four trials (training trials) the maze was open, allowing escape with either a left or right sided choice. The side preference of each pony on each of these training trials was recorded. In the second phase, the ability of the ponies to learn to turn left was measured. Starting with the fifth test (third day) the partition was used to block right side escape. When the pony correctly chose the left side for three consecutive trials, it was considered to have reached "criterion" or learned to turn left. Thereupon, the partition was moved for the particular pony to block left side escape, and the third phase of the study, measurement of the pony's ability to learn to turn right, began. The same learning criterion, three correct choices in a row, was applied to right side escape. Latency, time spent in the maze from the moment the pony crossed the threshold of the maze until the last hind foot was placed outside the maze, and errors (incorrect choices) were recorded. The ponies were allowed to correct an error, that is, if they initially chose the wrong side, they could retrace their steps and take the correct turn and obtain the food reward. The only punishment for making the incorrect choice was a delay in obtaining food. If the pony did not complete the maze in 5 min it was led through the exit of the maze and given a latency score of 300 s. All of the surviving mares (n = 11) and six of the foals (orphaned = 4; mothered = 2) were tested for social dominance. The six other foals had been euthanized as part of another study. Each of the mares was tested with every other mare, for a total of 55 pairings. Each foal was paired with the five other foals, for a total of 15 pairings. The ponies were tested before their regular evening feeding. ne bucket of grain was made available to the two ponies for 10 min. The pony that controlled the bucket for most of the test was considered to be dominant. The details of the method of dominance testing have been given by Houpt et al. (1978). The Mann Whitney U test (Siegel, 1956) was used to compare the times and the trials to criteria between orphan and mothered foals, be ~ tween colts and fillies, between mares and foals, between the reversal indexes, reversal index = trials to criterion to turn right (reversal) trials to criterion to turn left (original) of mares and foals, and between the reversal indexes of orphaned and mothered foals. The percentage side preference on the first five trials (four training trials and first trial when right side was blocked) was compared with 50% preference (Davis, 1962). Spearman rank order correlations were calculated between 1) trials to criteria for mares and trials to criteria for foals, 2) age and trials to criteria for mares, 3) age and trials to criteria of foals, 4) between age for all the ponies and trials to criteria, 5) weight and trials to critieria for mares, 6) weight and

MAZE LEARNING IN FALS 1029 I0 800 n- LLI I- n- o 0 I-- _1 n- I-. 5 r r V/,4 ///z r/11~ I/I/~,///~ r V/l/ iiii, LEFT TURN /'/// ~'///> /.///_,, / i / ~ i / / / ////,, N 1 / / ~ RIGHT TURN REVERSE n,n N Z n,i F- _J 600 400 200,/i/, Figure 1. Maze learning in orphan and mothered pony foals. The orphan foals are represented by the open column and the mothered foals by the cross hatched columns. The graph on the left, "trials to criteria," indicates the number of trials needed to learn to turn left'on three successive trials and the number of trials needed to learn the reverse, to turn right on three successive trials. The graph on the right illustrates the total time spent in the maze during the trials to criteria for both groups of ponies. Time for each trial was that elapsed from the moment the pony crossed the threshold of the entrance to the maze until the last hind foot was placed outside the exit. trials to criteria of foals, 7) dominance rank and trials to criteria and 8) weight and dominance rank (Siegel, 1956). Results and Discussion In the training trials and the first learning trial, the ponies showed a right side escape preference. In 73% of the trials, the mares turned right (P.05). The foals were more variable than the adults; in 60% of the trials, the foals turned right (P>.05). Grzimek (1949a) has also noted "handedness" in horses, that is, the horses he studied also showed a right side preference both in pawing and in lead taken on a straight path. In this study, those ponies that had an initial left side preference did not learn to turn left faster than those that initially turned right. There was no significant dlifference in the maze learning ability of orphaned and mothered foals. The least number of trials in which a foal could learn was three. The mothered foals learned to turn left, the first task taught, in 5.1 f.2 trials; orphan foals learned in 6 f.7 trials. The mothered foals learned the reverse task, turning right, in 6.2 +.6 trials; the orphans learned in 6.7 +.6 trials (figure 1). Although the orphan foals took no more trials to learn than their normally reared controls, they took longer to walk through the maze on the first training trial when both sides of the maze were open (orphaned = 184 + 42 s, mothered = 55 + 15 s, U = 7, P.05). n subsequent trials they were insignificantly slower than the normal foals (mean time for trials to criteria = 42 + 10 s/trial for ophaned; 27 + 4 s/trial for mothered foals; U = 20, P>.05). rphans, but not the mothered foals, traversed the maze more quickly with time (orphaned, r =.95, t= 4.47, P.05; mothered, r =.8, t = 1.1, P>.05). The slower exploration of a novel environment is similar to that observed in early weaned cats and dogs (Melzack and Scott, 1957; Seitz, 1959). It is interesting to note that there have been informal observations that early weaned foals do not race well. In contrast to early weaned dogs (Melzack and Scott, 1957) and cats (Seitz, 1959), which perform poorly in learning tasks, orphaned foals are not imj~aired in their ability to learn a

1030 HUPT ET AL. simple maze. There are several possible explanations for these differences among species in the effects of early weaning9 ne species may be more susceptible to the adverse effects of artificial rearing than another9 Precocial animals such as horses may be less affected than altricial animals such as cats. The most likely explanation, however, is differences in experimental methods among the studies. The cats and dogs were isolated from conspecifics, as well as being weaned early. In addition, the tasks imposed were different. The dogs were learning to avoid pain, and the cats were learning to make a visual discrimination for a food reward, while the foals in this study were learning to follow a certain route for a food reward9 Species differences in effects of early weaning on visual acuity and(or) motor function might also affect the results. Perhaps, if the task had been more complicated or difficult, differences between foals might have been found9 The mothers of the foals learned the initial task in as few trials as their offspring, but were slower to learn the reverse task. The mares learned the left turn in 5.9 -+.7 trials. They took significandy more trials (9.8 + 1.4, U = 25, P.04) to learn the reversal. The mares were more variable than the foals; the standard deviation was larger (t = 2.81, P.05). The foals took significantly fewer trials (6.4 -+.4) to reach criteria than the mares (U = 33, P.05). The reversal index or ratio of the number of trials or errors to criterion on the original learning discrimination has been suggested as a measure of learning ability that is relatively independent of task difficulty. For most animals the index is greater than one, indicating that it is more difficult to learn the reversal than the original task (Hinde, 1970). The reversal index was 1.2 -+.2 for the orphan foals, 1.2 -+.02 for the mothered foals and 1.7 -+.2 for the mares9 There is a possibility that the opportunity to eat was more rewarding for the foals than for the mares, but that would be a difficult hypothesis to test. All were equally food deprived. The offspring of mothers that learned in a small number of trials did not necessarily learn in a small number of trials. There was no significant correlation between the trials to criteria for the mare and the trials to criteria for the foal, whether learning to turn left, learning to turn right or total trials to criteria were compared. The age, weight, dominance rank, time/trial, and trials to criteria of the mares, as well as the r~ o b~ r~ 0 "~ 'r~ M ~'~, 0 9?'~ ~ 0 0 0 0 ~

MAZE LEARNING IN FALS 1031 trials to criteria, time/trial, body weight and dominance rank of the foals are presented in table 1. There was a correlation (r =.65, P.05) between age of the foals and trials to criteria, but not between age of the mare and her trials to criteria. Therefore, immature horses may tend to learn a maze faster than adult horses. This tentative conclusion is supported by other studies on horses. Mader and Price (1980) studied visual discrimination in Quarterhorses and Thoroughbreds and also found that trainability was negatively correlated with age. The present study extends their observation to another learning situation, maze learning, and to another type of equid, grade ponies. Kratzer (1969) found that younger pigs also learned to avoid shock in fewer trials than older pigs. A similar trend has been noted in rats (Denenberg and Kline, 1958). Although the foals learned in fewer trials than their mothers, they spent significantly more time in the maze (foals = 36 + 5 s; mares = 13.2 + 2 s, U = 32, P.05). The mares may have failed to learn because they reached food very quickly despite their errors. There was no correlation between body weight and rate of maze learning in either the foals or the mares, but there was a significant positive correlation between body weight and rank of the mares in the hierarchy (r =.7, P.05). A positive relationship may indicate that larger horses are dominant or that dominant horses have more access to food and, therefore, gain more weight. The cause and effect relationship of social dominance and weight has not yet been determined. Previous studies of equine dominance hierarchies have indicated a tendency for heavier horses to be dominant (Grzimek, 1949b; Montgomery, 1957; Houpt et al., 1978). It is difficult to measure learning ability without building a maze or operant conditioning device or using painful stimuli. It is easier to determine dominance and it would, therefore, be helpful to be able to predict learning ability by measuring social dominance. Unfortunately, social dominance does not appear to predict learning ability. Haag et al. (1980) and Mader and Price (1980) have also observed that there was no correlation between rank in the dominance hierarchy and learning ability. The results on social dominance in the foals must be considered tentative because they were obtained on a small number of animals before a stable hierarchy would have been expected to form (Grz~mek, 1949b). Nevertheless, it is interesting to note that the mothered foals were dominant in the same order as their mothers, but the orphan foals were not. In a previous study of normally reared horses, the offspring of dominant mares were found to rank high in dominance among their own age group (Houpt and Wolski, 1980). These results suggest, but do not prove, that horses may learn to dominate rather than inherit the tendency. This hypothesis should be rigorously tested with large numbers of cross-fostered or orphan horses. In conclusion, it would appear that lack of normal maternal care does not adversely affect the maze learning ability of foals. The orphan foals initially moved through the maze more slowly, but made no more errors than mothered foals. Age does seem to be an important factor in equine learning, because adult mares were more variable and made significantly more errors-that is, took more trials to learn a simple reversal-than their foals. There was no correlation between the time and trials to criteria of the mare and that of her offspring. Rank in a social dominance order was not related to maze learning ability, but heavier mares tended to be socially dominant. These studies of learning ability of horses should be extended to other breeds and more complex tasks. Literature Cited Burton, S. C., H. F. Hintz, M. J. Kemen and D. F. Holmes. 1981. Lyophilized hyperimmune equine serum as a source of antibodies for neonatal foals. Amer. J. Vet. Res. 42:308. Davies, V. 1962. Rapid method for determining significance of the difference between two percentages. Washington Agr. Exp. Sta. Circular 18. Denenberg, V. H. and N. J. Kline. 1958. The relationship between age and avoidance learning in the hooded rat. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 51:488. Fiske, J. C. and G. D. Potter. 1979. Discrimination reversal learning in yearling horses. J. Anita. Sci. 49:583. Gardner, L. P. 1937. The response of horses in a discrimination problem. J. Comp. Psychol. 23:13. Grzimek, B. 1949a. Rechts- nnd Linkshandigkeit bei Pferden, Papageien und Affen. Z. Tierpsychol. 6:406. Grzimek, B. 1949b. Rangordnungsversuche mit Pferden. Z Tierpsychol. 6:455. Haag, E. L., R. Rudman and K. A. Houpt. 1980. Avoidance, maze learning and social dominance in ponies. J. Anita. Sci. 50:329. Hinde, R. A. 1970. Animal Behaviour. McGraw-Hill Book Co., New York. p 660. Houpt, K. A., K. Law and V. Martinisi. 1978. Dominance hierarchies in domestic horses. Appl. Anim. Ethol. 4:273.

1032 HUPT ET AL. Houpt, K. A. and T. R. Wolski. 1980. Stability of equine hierarchies and the prevention of dominance related aggression. Equine Vet. J. 12:18. Kratzer, D. D. 1969. Effects of age on avoidance learning in pigs. J. Anita. Sci. 28:175. Kratzer, D. D., W. M. Netherland, R. E. Pulse and J. P. Baker. 1977. Maze learning in quarter horses. J. Anita. Sci. 46:896. Mader, D. R. and E.. Price. 1980. Discrimination learning in horses: Effects of breed, age and social dominance. J. Anita. Sci. 50:962. Melzack, R. and T. T. Scott. 1957. The effects of early experience on the response to pain. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 50:155. Montgomery, G. G. 1957. Some aspects of the sociality of the domestic horse. Trans. Kansas Acad. Sci. 60:419. Seitz, P.F.D. 1959. Infantile experience and adult behavior in animal subjects. II. Age of separation from the mother and adult behavior in the cat. Psychosom. Med. 21 : 353. Siegel, S. 1956. Nonparametric Statistics for the Behavioral Sciences. McGraw-Hill Book Co., New York.