Effects on Auditory Attention and Walking While Texting with a Smartphone and Walking on Stairs

Similar documents
Proposal for a System of Mutual Support Among Passengers Trapped Inside a Train

Pedestrian smartphone overuse and inattentional blindness: an observational study in Taipei, Taiwan

Effect of Different Distractions on Driving Performance for Drivers Using a Touch Screen

The Influence of Heavy Truck Egress Tactics on Ground Reaction Force

Shoe-shaped Interface for Inducing a Walking Cycle

Evaluation of cross-walk timing and the application of a standard crossing light timing formula

The Effects of Dual-Task Interference and Response Strategy on Stop or Go Decisions to Yellow Light Changes

Covert Texting During Simulated Driving Maneuvers: Effects of Head-Up versus Head-Down Posture

DOI /HORIZONS.B P23 UDC : (497.11) PEDESTRIAN CROSSING BEHAVIOUR AT UNSIGNALIZED CROSSINGS 1

Pedestrian traffic flow operations on a platform: observations and comparison with simulation tool SimPed

Legendre et al Appendices and Supplements, p. 1

Fatigue Monitoring Device for Monitoring Haul Truck Operator Alertness. 2 The Challenge

Nervous System: Reaction Time Student Version

Factors Affecting Phonotaxis in Male House Crickets, Acheta domesticus

Analysis of Variance. Copyright 2014 Pearson Education, Inc.

Mutual and asynchronous anticipation and action in sports as globally competitive

Keywords: multiple linear regression; pedestrian crossing delay; right-turn car flow; the number of pedestrians;

Factorial Analysis of Variance

The Effect of Pavement Marking on Speed. Reduction in Exclusive Motorcycle Lane. in Malaysia

VISUOMOTOR CONTROL OF STRAIGHT AND BREAKING GOLF PUTTS 1

Prediction of Crossing Driveways of a Distracted Pedestrian from Walking Characteristics

Subjective Evaluation for the Driving Conditions at Signalized Intersection and Roundabout by Before-After Analysis

Jonghyun Ko. Wondea Jung, Jinkyun Park

Evaluation of Standing Stability and Reaching Postures on a Stepladder for Occupational Fall Prevention

The effect of different backpack loading systems on trunk forward lean angle during walking among college students

Remote Towers: Videopanorama Framerate Requirements Derived from Visual Discrimination of Deceleration During Simulated Aircraft Landing

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

Analysis of Gait Characteristics Changes in Normal Walking and Fast Walking Of the Elderly People

COLLISION AVOIDANCE SYSTEM FOR BUSES, MANAGING PEDESTRIAN DETECTION AND ALERTS NEAR BUS STOPS

Cyclists gaze behavior in urban space: an eye-tracking experiment on bicycle facilities.

A Case Study of Leadership in Women s Intercollegiate Softball. By: DIANE L. GILL and JEAN L. PERRY

Biology Project. Investigate and compare the quantitative effects of changing,

AGE, SKILL, AND HAZARD PERCEPTION IN DRIVING

Available online at ScienceDirect. Procedia Engineering 112 (2015 )

Walking aids based on wearable/ubiquitous computing aiming at pedestrian s intelligent transport systems

Evaluation of Pedestrian and Cyclist Warning Systems for Trucks

WORLD SCIENTIFIC CONGRESS OF GOLF. ST. ANDREWS, SCOTLAND TH JULY 2002 SCIENCE AND GOLF IV PROCEEDINGS OF THE WORLD SCIENTIFIC CONGRESS OF GOLF

Results of mathematical modelling the kinetics of gaseous exchange through small channels in micro dischargers

An analysis on feasibility of park & cycle ride system in a Japanese local city

Quantifying the Bullwhip Effect of Multi-echelon System with Stochastic Dependent Lead Time

Modelling of French High Speed Train Evacuation

THREE DIMENSIONAL STRUCTURES OF FLOW BEHIND A

Investigation of driving behaviour reflecting drivers risk anticipation for pedestrian collision risk assessment of right-turns at intersections

Conditioned Alarm Behavior in Fathead Minnows (Pimephales promelas) and Test Their Ability

A COMPARATIVE STUDY OF PHYSICAL DIFFERENCES BETWEEN ATHLETES OF SELECTED EVENTS IN TRACK AND FIELD

A Research on Visual Analysis of Badminton for Skill Learning

Data Mining Data is logged every 3 hours 11 parameters : 1. Number of call 2. Correspondents and calls pattern 3. Call duration 4. Circle of friend 5.

Safety Behavior for Cycling : Application Theory of Planned Behavior


Internet Use Among Illinois Hunters: A Ten Year Comparison

The effects of a suspended-load backpack on gait

Grip Force and Heart Rate Responses to Manual Carrying Tasks: Effects of Material, Weight, and Base Area of the Container

Covert Texting During Simulated Driving Maneuvers Effects of Head-Up versus Head-Down Posture

Analysis of Backward Falls Caused by Accelerated Floor Movements Using a Dummy

Effects of Smartphone Use on Behavior While Walking

Impact Points and Their Effect on Trajectory in Soccer

A location model for pedestrian crossings in arterial streets

A COMPARISON OF SELECTED BIOMECHANICAL PARAMETERS OF FRONT ROW SPIKE BETWEEN SHORT SET AND HIGH SET BALL

HOW DO ELDERLY PEDESTRIANS PERCEIVE HAZARDS IN THE STREET?

One-factor ANOVA by example

Clothing for Improving Mine Worker Visibility

Directional guidance from audible pedestrian signals for street crossing

Visual Attention Failures during Turns at Intersections: An On-road Study *

The automobile is a complex system in which humans play an important role. Driving is

MRI-2: Integrated Simulation and Safety

Development of a Non-Destructive Test Machine for Testing Tunnel Lining Concrete

Available online at ScienceDirect. Analysis of Pedestrian Clearance Time at Signalized Crosswalks in Japan

University of Groningen

Simple countermeasure for preventing falls from portable ladders and residential roofs

Distance between Speed Humps and Pedestrian Crossings: Does it Matter?

Toshihisa Sato, 1 Motoyuki Akamatsu, 1 Toru Shibata, 2 Shingo Matsumoto, 2 Naoki Hatakeyama, 2 and Kazunori Hayama 2. 1.

Investigating Selection and Reading Performance on a Mobile Phone while Walking

ESTIMATION OF THE EFFECT OF AUTONOMOUS EMERGENCY BRAKING SYSTEMS FOR PEDESTRIANS ON REDUCTION IN THE NUMBER OF PEDESTRIAN VICTIMS

BIOL 101L: Principles of Biology Laboratory

A Study of the Normal Turbulence Model in IEC

BODY FORM INFLUENCES ON THE DRAG EXPERIENCED BY JUNIOR SWIMMERS. Australia, Perth, Australia

Evaluation of shared use of bicycles and pedestrians in Japan

Every time a driver is distracted,

Towards determining absolute velocity of freestyle swimming using 3-axis accelerometers

Pedestrian Scenario Design and Performance Assessment in Driving Simulations

Level 3 Mathematics and Statistics (Statistics), 2013

Pedestrian self-reported exposure to distraction by smart phones while walking and crossing the road

APPROACH RUN VELOCITIES OF FEMALE POLE VAULTERS

Available online at ScienceDirect. Procedia Manufacturing 3 (2015 )

PROPAGATION OF LONG-PERIOD WAVES INTO AN ESTUARY THROUGH A NARROW INLET

The impact of freediving on psychomotor performance and blood catecholamine concentration

Efficacy of Static and Dynamic Distance Perception on Kumite Performance in Karate

Module 3 Developing Timing Plans for Efficient Intersection Operations During Moderate Traffic Volume Conditions

Tennis Plots: Game, Set, and Match

c 2015 Sarah E. Banducci

IMAGE-BASED FIELD OBSERVATION OF INFRAGRAVITY WAVES ALONG THE SWASH ZONE. Yoshimitsu Tajima 1

COMPARATIVE STUDY OF KICKING PERFORMANCE BASED ON DIFFERENT KIND OF SHOES

Comparing Evaluation Methods for Encumbrance and Walking on Interaction with Touchscreen Mobile Devices

ICBC Driving Habits DRIVING HABITS AND BEHAVIOURS

A Traffic Operations Method for Assessing Automobile and Bicycle Shared Roadways

Analysis of Curling Team Strategy and Tactics using Curling Informatics

Modeling and simulation of multiple personal mobility. vehicles in pedestrian flows using personal space

IMPROVED RICE METHOD FOR DETERMINING THEORETICAL MAXIMUM SPECIFIC GRAVITY OF ASPHALT PAVING MIXTURES

Aerodynamic characteristics around the stalling angle of the discus using a PIV

Ocean Inter-annual Variability: El Niño and La Niña. How does El Niño influence the oceans and climate patterns?

Transcription:

Effects on Auditory Attention and Walking While Texting with a Smartphone and Walking on Stairs Shigeru Haga (&), Kanae Fukuzawa, Eri Kido, Yoshinori Sudo, and Azuri Yoshida Rikkyo University, Niiza, Saitama, Japan Abstract. Effects of texting with a smartphone while walking on stairs in a building were investigated. Twenty-four students performed an auditory detection task for 60 s while using (texting condition) or not using (holding condition) an iphone 5s. Half of the participants (walking group) performed the signal detection task while walking up the stairs and half of the participants (standing group) performed the task while standing still. Results showed that participants in the texting condition responded significantly more slowly to the targets and missed more targets than in the holding condition. Participants in the walking group missed more auditory targets. We also found a large effect size of smartphone use on a walking performance. Results of our present study can be used as evidence to show the risk of using smartphones, especially on stairs. Keywords: Cell phone Texting Pedestrian Inattention Safety 1 Introduction Our previous studies provided empirical evidence of inattention by pedestrians using cell phones. Among them, Masuda et al. [1] compared reaction time to visual and auditory stimuli while walking with a cell phone (feature phone or smartphone of their own) in hand under four experimental conditions: texting, conversation, orally repeating words heard, and control (just holding a phone). Results showed that reaction times to either visual or auditory stimuli were significantly longer under the three cell phone-use conditions than under the control condition. Moreover, participants who used smartphones responded more slowly to visual stimuli under the texting condition than those using feature (standard cell) phones. Haga et al. [2] requested university students with an iphone 5s to text a message, watch a video, play a game, or just to hold the phone (control condition) in addition to performing visual and auditory detection tasks at the same time. The number of right footsteps that missed the line marking the walking route was greater under cell phone-use conditions than under the control condition. Mean reaction times for both visual and auditory targets were significantly longer under cell phone-use conditions than under the control condition. Springer International Publishing Switzerland 2016 C. Stephanidis (Ed.): HCII 2016 Posters, Part I, CCIS 617, pp. 186 191, 2016. DOI: 10.1007/978-3-319-40548-3_31

Effects on Auditory Attention and Walking While Texting 187 Injury reports show the occurrence of a number of accidents involving cell phone users in railway stations. Commuters use smartphones on board and continue to use them after getting off the train. Some people check the train schedule by phone while walking through stations before getting on a train. Others use the phone in a waiting room or on a platform for communication or amusement, and then start walking to get on a train. Many people now use their smartphones to communicate with others by email, LINE, Facebook, Twitter, etc., all of which involve texting. The purpose of this study was to measure quantitatively the effects of texting with a smartphone on auditory attention and walking. In this study, we asked our experimental participants to walk on stairs. This differs from previous studies that dealt with cell phone use while walking on a flat floor or a treadmill (e.g. [1 4]). 2 Methods Twenty-four undergraduate students participated in the experiment and were divided into two groups according to whether they walked up stairs or stayed still while they performed an auditory detection task for 60 s while using (texting condition) or not using (holding condition) a smartphone (APPLE iphone 5s). The detection task was to respond to designated target signals as quickly as possible by clicking the button held in the hand that was not holding the phone. Auditory stimuli were presented through a pair of earphones (Pioneer SE-E11-Z1) once every second for a duration of 500 ms. The target stimulus for reaction was a higher pitch (460 Hz) among the tones (440 Hz). The button for reaction (Kokuyo ELA-FP1) was connected wirelessly to a laptop computer (SONY VAIO Fit 11) that controlled the stimulus presentation. Every reaction time was recorded. After a 60-s trial, the participants rated the subjective workload of the task with the Japanese version of NASA-TLX [5]. As with the original NASA-TLX [6], the rating scale consisted of 6 subscales: mental demand, physical demand, temporal demand, participant s performance, effort, and frustration. Participants were asked to describe their travel in detail from their house to the university on the Notes of the iphone under the texting condition. Under the holding condition they just held the phone in hand. The experiment took place on a staircase in an 8-story building with a landing between floors. Participants in the walking group were instructed to walk up the stairs at a safe speed and to step with the right foot on the white line drawn in the middle of the stairway. We recorded participants steps as they walked on the stairs with a video camera (SONY HDR-SR8). The participants wore a hard hat such as worn by cyclists and knee and elbow pads to prevent injury. Figure 1 is a photo taken of a walking trial. 3 Results 3.1 Data Analysis Data obtained from two participants in the walking group were excluded from analysis due to noncompliance to the instructions. In order to match the number of subjects, two

188 S. Haga et al. participants in the standing group were chosen at random and removed from our data pool. Hence, data from twenty participants were used for analysis. Within each behavior (walking or standing) group, five participants performed the texting condition followed by the holding condition, and the remaining five performed the task in the opposite order. Two-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) was applied for dependent variables observed in the detection task and workload ratings. The factor of smartphone use was a within-subject independent variable and the factor of behavior was a between-subject independent variable. Student s t-test was used for variables related to walking performance in the walking group. Cohen s d for walking performance and partial eta-squared (ηp 2 ) for the detection task and the workload ratings were calculated to evaluate the effect size of independent variables. Fig. 1. Photo of a walking trial 3.2 Results of Detection Task Mean reaction times to auditory targets are shown in Fig. 2. Interaction between the two factors was not significant. Main effect of behavior was not significant but that of smartphone use was significant (F(1, 18) = 10.64, p <.001, ηp 2 =.37). The participants responded significantly more slowly to the targets in the texting condition than in the holding condition. Mean number of missed targets is shown in Fig. 3. Interaction between the two factors was not significant (F(1, 18) =.16, p >.05, ηp 2 =.11). Main effects were significant both for behavior (F(1, 18) = 15.84, p <.01, ηp 2 =.47) and smartphone use (F(1, 18) = 10.68, p <.01, ηp 2 =.37). The participants in the walking group missed more targets than those in the standing group, and they missed more targets under the texting condition than under the holding condition.

Effects on Auditory Attention and Walking While Texting 189 Fig. 2. Mean reaction time to auditory signals. Error bars show standard errors Fig. 3. Mean number of missed auditory signals. Error bars show standard errors 3.3 Results of Workload Ratings The AWWL score [7] was calculated from ratings on 6 subscales of the Japanese version of NASA-TLX [5] using the weight of 6 for the highest-rated subscale, 5 for the second highest, etc. The result is shown in Fig. 4. Interaction between the two factors was not significant (F(1, 18) =.01, p >.05, ηp 2 =.001). Main effect of behavior was not significant but that of smartphone use was significant (F(1, 18) = 58.58, p <.001, ηp 2 =.77). Participants rated the task more demanding after trials with texting than after trials in the holding condition. 3.4 Results of Walking Performance There were significant differences in the number of floors walked up (t = 3.34, df =9, p <.01,d =.80) and the number of total footsteps taken (t = 3.13, df =9,p <.05, d =.51) between the texting and holding conditions. Participants walked a shorter distance and took fewer steps when they texted with a phone while walking.

190 S. Haga et al. Fig. 4. Subjective mental workload rated on the Japanese version of NASA-TLX. Error bars show standard errors Difference in the number of right footsteps that missed the line was not significant between the holding and texting conditions (t = 1.47, df =9,p >.05, d =.69). We calculated the rate of step-outs by dividing the number of step-outs by the total number of steps taken. As shown in Fig. 5, those in the texting condition stepped out at a higher rate. The difference between the smartphone use conditions was not statistically significant (t = 1.83, df =9,p >.05, d =.89), but the effect size was large. Fig. 5. Rate of steps that missed the line out of total number of steps walked. Error bars show standard errors 4 Discussion As shown above, participants responded significantly more slowly to auditory targets, missed more stimuli, and felt the task more demanding when they were texting with a phone than when they were just holding a phone. These results demonstrated deterioration of the user s attention when smartphone texting. The facts that participants in the walking group missed more targets and the effect size of smartphone use was large

Effects on Auditory Attention and Walking While Texting 191 regarding the number of step-outs suggested that walking on stairs makes people less attentive to auditory stimuli and to their own actions. However, the effects were not amplified when walking on stairs since no significant interaction between smartphone use and behavior was found. Smartphone use seemed to equally deteriorate attention no matter whether participants were standing or walking. It might be a consequence of our instruction to walk safely. Participants walked slowly. Since the risk of injury caused by inattention is much greater for people walking on stairs than people standing still, our present results can be used as evidence to show the risk of using smartphones, especially on stairs. References 1. Masuda, K., Takahashi, H., Haga, S.: Hokouchuuno keitaidenwa shiyouga chuuito hokouni oyobosu eikyou (Effects of cell phone use while walking on pedestrians attention and walking). In: Proceedings for the 53rd Annual Meeting of the Japanese Ergonomics Society, pp. 206 207 (2012). (Japanese) 2. Haga, S., Sano, A., Sekine, Y., Sato, H., Yamaguchi, S., Masuda, K.: Effects of using a smart phone on pedestrians attention and walking. Procedia Manuf. 3, 2574 2580 (2015) 3. Nasar, J., Hecht, P., Wener, R.: Mobile telephones, distracted attention, and pedestrian safety. Accid. Anal. Prev. 40, 69 75 (2008) 4. Hyman Jr., I.E., Boss, S.M., Wise, B.M., Mckenzie, K.E., Caggiano, J.M.: Did you see the unicycling clown? Inattentional blindness while walking and talking on a cell phone. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 24, 597 607 (2010) 5. Haga, S., Mizukami, N.: Japanese version of NASA task load index: sensitivity of its workload score to difficulty of three different laboratory tasks. Japan. J. Ergonomics 32,71 80 (1996). (Japanese with English abstract) 6. Hart, S.G., Staveland, L.E.: Development of NASA-TLX (task load index); results of empirical and theoretical research. In: Hancock, P.A., Meshkati, N. (eds.) Human Mental Workload, pp. 139 183. North Holland, Amsterdam (1988) 7. Miyake, S., Kumashiro, M.: Subjective mental workload assessment technique: an introduction to NASA-TLX and SWAT and a proposal of simple scoring methods. Japan. J. Ergonomics 29, 399 408 (1993). (Japanese with English abstract)