The Effect of Bicycle Color on Visibility

Similar documents
The Quantification of Bicyclist Conspicuity Using Eye-Tracking Technology

Children s expectations and beliefs toward the relative safety of riding bicycles at night

Cyclist-reported habits of helmet usage and differences in riding postures by using helmets

MRI-2: Integrated Simulation and Safety

STATIC AND DYNAMIC EVALUATION OF THE DRIVER SPEED PERCEPTION AND SELECTION PROCESS

The automobile is a complex system in which humans play an important role. Driving is

People killed and injured per million hours spent travelling, Motorcyclist Cyclist Driver Car / van passenger

Visual Attention Failures during Turns at Intersections: An On-road Study *

the Ministry of Transport is attributed as the source of the material

LECTUR 10 CHARACTERISTICS OF THE DRIVER, THE PEDESTRIAN, THE VEHICLE AND THE ROAD. One problem that faces traffic and transportation engineers is:

Safety and Active Transport. Dr. Maureen Carew, Medical Officer of Health Renfrew County and District Health Unit May 30, 2014

Deaths/injuries in motor vehicle crashes per million hours spent travelling, July 2007 June 2011 (All ages) Mode of travel

In 2014, the number of traffic fatalities in the United States reached its lowest level at. Bicycle Collisions. Effective in Reducing

The Effect of Pavement Marking on Speed. Reduction in Exclusive Motorcycle Lane. in Malaysia

Strategies for Sharing the Road with Other Users

AGE, SKILL, AND HAZARD PERCEPTION IN DRIVING

Elderly cyclists opinions on safe and joyful cycling

Baseline Survey of New Zealanders' Attitudes and Behaviours towards Cycling in Urban Settings

THE DEVELOPMENT OF MALAYSIAN HIGHWAY RAIL LEVEL CROSSING SAFETY SYSTEMS: A PROPOSED RESEARCH FRAMEWORK. Siti Zaharah Ishak

To Illuminate or Not to Illuminate: Roadway Lighting as It Affects Traffic Safety at Intersections

Why Zone In on Speed Reduction?

Safer Cycling: How the City of Vancouver is Proactively Improving Cycling Safety

Prediction model of cyclist s accident probability in the City of Malang

Bicycle - Motor Vehicle Collisions on Controlled Access Highways in Arizona

MTCF. Michigan Traffic Crash Facts FACT SHEETS

MRI-2: Integrated Simulation and Safety

Risk on the Road. Pedestrians, Cyclists and Motorcyclists August 2015

MTCF. Michigan Traffic Crash Facts FACT SHEETS

Advance Yield Markings Reduce Motor Vehicle/Pedestrian. Conflicts at Multilane Crosswalks with an Uncontrolled Approach.

MCIWEST-MCB CAMP PENDLETON Critical Days of Summer Newsletter. Week 11: 31 Jul - 04 Aug Bicycle Safety

EAST VILLAGE SHOPPERS STUDY A SNAPSHOT OF TRAVEL AND SPENDING PATTERNS OF RESIDENTS AND VISITORS IN THE EAST VILLAGE

Every time a driver is distracted,

The Effects of Dual-Task Interference and Response Strategy on Stop or Go Decisions to Yellow Light Changes

Low Level Cycle Signals used as repeaters of the main traffic signals Appendices

Clothing for Improving Mine Worker Visibility

Low Level Cycle Signals with an early release Appendices

BICYCLE SAFETY OBSERVATION STUDY 2014

INSURANCE INSTITUTE FOR HIGHWAY SAFETY 1005 NORTH GLEBE ROAD ARLINGTON, VA PHONE 703/ FAX 703/

Cyclist Safety in Urban Areas

BICYCLE INFRASTRUCTURE PREFERENCES A CASE STUDY OF DUBLIN

When composing the study, our primary source for content analysis was a study

Use of Throw Distances of Pedestrians and Bicyclists as Part of a Scientific Accident Reconstruction Method 1

ITARDA INFORMATION. No.128. Special feature

Bicycle Collision Analysis Using a Vehicle Driving Simulator MOVIC-T4

TYPES OF CYCLING. Figure 1: Types of Cycling by Gender (Actual) Figure 2: Types of Cycling by Gender (%) 65% Chi-squared significance test results 65%

Traffic Safety Facts 2007 Data

The 2002 Pedestrian Safety Awareness Campaign

U.S. Bicycling Participation Study

Determining bicycle infrastructure preferences A case study of Dublin

Safety Assessment of Installing Traffic Signals at High-Speed Expressway Intersections

FYG Backing for Work Zone Signs

Evaluation of shared use of bicycles and pedestrians in Japan

A traffic safety guide concerning bicyclists, in-line skaters, pedestrians, non-motorized scooter operators, motorcyclists and motorists.

Double Pair Comparisons PART III AGE & GENDER. Age and Crash Risk. Subject & control groups Relative risk or rate. Relative Accident Involvement Ratio

Bhagwant N. Persaud* Richard A. Retting Craig Lyon* Anne T. McCartt. May *Consultant to the Insurance Institute for Highway Safety

Bicycle Helmet Use Among Winnipeg Cyclists January 2012

North Carolina. Bicycle Crash Facts Prepared for

Synthesis of Safety For Traffic Operations. Leanna Belluz Transport Canada. Gerry Forbes, M.Eng., P.Eng., P.T.O.E. Intus Road Safety Engineering Inc.

National Bicycle and Pedestrian Documentation Project INSTRUCTIONS

INTERSECTION DESIGN. Bicycle Facility Workshop Intersections 4-1

Evaluation of Pedestrian and Cyclist Warning Systems for Trucks

Analysis of the Red Light Camera Program in Garden Grove, CA By Jay Beeber, Executive Director, Safer Streets L.A., Member ITE

A Traffic Operations Method for Assessing Automobile and Bicycle Shared Roadways

Analysis and Countermeasure Research on the Death Rate of the Vulnerable Road Users in Traffic Accidents. Ying MEI

Parental Responsibilities

Lake Charles Harley Owner Group Chapter #1686 September Newsletter

Red-Light Behavior between Motor Vehicles and Bicycles By: Aaron Cole, Stephanie Benston, Philip Cohoe, and Stacy Harris

Interaction with the audience: expert, citizens, bike communities

An application of the Safe System Approach to a set of self-reported cycling crashes

Bicycling Simulator Calibration: Proposed Framework

Design Considerations for Complete Streets. The Kiewit Center for Infrastructure and Transportation

Lane Area Transportation Safety and Security Plan Vulnerable Users Focus Group

Misuse of Seatbelts Among Females Aged Years

New in the MUTCD: The Flashing Yellow Arrow Presented at the 57 th Annual Traffic and Safety Conference May 17, 2006

Peninsula Velo Cycling Club. Riding Etiquette and Expectations on the Road

Alberta. Traffic Collision Statistics. Office of Traffic Safety Transportation Services Division May 2017

INSURANCE INSTITUTE FOR HIGHWAY SAFETY 1005 NORTH GLEBE ROAD ARLINGTON, VA PHONE 703/ FAX 703/

Department of Transportation

the Ministry of Transport is attributed as the source of the material

Look Up! Positioning-based Pedestrian Risk Awareness. Shubham Jain

Saturation Flow Rate, Start-Up Lost Time, and Capacity for Bicycles at Signalized Intersections

Transportation Issues Poll for New York City

H 232 Bicycle Safety Report

Post impact trajectory of vehicles at rural intersections

In-Road Warning Lights: Revisiting Pedestrian Crosswalk Application Faisal Hamood, Emelinda M. Parentela, Zaki Mustafa and Crystal Killian

Identify the letter of the choice that best completes the statement or answers the question.

Safety and Design Alternatives for Two-Way Stop-Controlled Expressway Intersections

Safety Behavior for Cycling : Application Theory of Planned Behavior

Investigating Commute Mode and Route Choice Variability in Jakarta using multi-day GPS Data

Chapter 2 Defensive Driving

Urban planning and traffic safety at night

304B Gordon- TTU Lubbock, TX April 6 th, 2011 Mr. Tyler Patton Office of the President 4952 Matador Avenue Lubbock, TX Dear Tyler Patton:

SOMERSET ROAD SAFETY PARTNERSHIP CASUALTY REVIEW Working together to reduce casualties

MEMORANDUM. City Constituents. Leilani Schwarcz, Vision Zero Surveillance Epidemiologist, SFDPH

Bicycle and Pedestrian Level of Service Evaluation

Advanced driver assistance systems status and future potential

Relationship of Road Lane Width to Safety for Urban and Suburban Arterials

A Conceptual Approach for Using the UCF Driving Simulator as a Test Bed for High Risk Locations

National Bicycle and Pedestrian Documentation Project INSTRUCTIONS

SECTION 1. The current state of global road safety

Transcription:

The Effect of Bicycle Color on Visibility Ellen Szubski Department of Psychology Latonya Housie Department of Computer Science Ryan Monaghan Department of Business Eric Switzer Department of Computer Science Abstract In 2015, 45,000 bicyclists were injured from collisions with vehicles [5]. Using a Gazepoint GP3 eye tracker, we conducted an experiment to determine which color bicycle helps cyclists become more visible to drivers during the day in an urban environment. 9 participants with good vision viewed images of different colored bicycles (black, blue, yellow, white) from near and far distances on a computer screen. Although no significant differences were found, the near blue bicyclist had the lowest and best reaction time of all of the colors, followed by the black bicycle, the yellow bicycle, and the white bicycle according to group means. No significant reaction was found between distance and bicycle color on the total time spent on an image. However, we did find that the yellow and black bicycles appear to be robust to distance. This pattern could suggest that regardless of distance, the yellow and black bicycles are found in the same amount of time. Also, we found that the more experienced drivers tended to respond faster to seeing the yellow bicycle images indicating a possible learned association of the color yellow with bicycle safety. This study had the limitations of small sample size and eye tracker calibration errors. Future studies should expand upon these preliminary findings to determine the relationship between bicycle color and distance in terms of bicycle safety. Keywords bicycle safety, eyetracking, driving safety, bicycle visibility 1 Introduction More and more people have started cycling for health and transportation reasons. With more people on the road, bicyclist and car collisions are on the rise and becoming a significant problem. In 2015, an estimated 45,000 people were injured in bicyclist/vehicle collisions [5]. Of these collisions, there was an increase of 729 bicyclist fatalities to 818 bicyclist fatalities in 2015 [5]. 47% of bicyclist fatalities occurred during the daytime and 70% of fatalities occur in urban environments [5]. Drivers often report not noticing a bicyclist until it was too late to avoid a collision [6], which makes it vital for bicyclist to increase their visibility and for the driver to see the cyclist to as soon as possible. There are various ways to increase bicyclist visibility during the daytime through active means like tail lights and fluorescent colored clothing, meaning that the cyclist much actively put a visibility aid on their body or bicycle to increase their visibility. The National Highway Traffic Safety Administration recommends that a bicyclist should wear fluorescent or brightly colored clothing during the day to be better noticed by drivers and to increase bicyclist visibility [5]. Both brightly colored clothing and taillights require the cyclist to have the forethought to put something on either their bicycle or to wear something. A significant challenge to cyclist visibility are the lack of education on the significance of being seen by drivers, the detrimental effects of wearing dark clothes, and refusal to wear visibility aids such as reflectors for the purpose of being fashionable [2]. Bicyclists need to be aware of how common vehicle and bicycle collisions are and what visibility aids can be implemented to avoid collisions. Hagel et al. (2007), found in an observational study that around a third of the cyclists observed actually wore a bright color on the majority of the torso while riding. The vast majority of cyclists observed were not wearing a bright color, which could have increased their visibility. Coloring the bicycle instead of having the cyclist complete an additional action, could offer a passive solution to increasing bicyclist visibility. A brightly colored bicycle color would not require an additional action on the cyclist's part to remember before their ride and may offer a solution to increasing bicyclist visibility. To our knowledge, no published research has addressed the specific daytime visibility benefits of various colored bicycles. The purpose of this study is to determine which color bicycle helps bicyclist become more visible to drivers during the day in an urban environment. Participants will view images photographed from near and far distances of a cyclist on various colored bicycles on a computer screen. Their eye-movements will be tracked in order to gain insight on when the observer locates the bicyclist using a Gazepoint GP3 Eye-Tracker. It is predicted that the fluorescent yellow colored bicycle will be found faster than the black colored bicycle. We predict that the blue bicycle will be found faster than the black bicycle. It is also predicted that the bicyclist photographed from a closer distance will be found more quickly than the bicyclist photographed at a further distance. 2 Background 1

There is a small but growing amount of research that addresses various visibility aids such as fluorescent clothing that riders may utilize to increase their visibility during daytime. Schieber, Willan and Schlorholtz (2006) conducted a study to determine the attentional conspicuity of fluorescent signs verses not fluorescent signs using eye trackers. In this study, participants were fitted with eye trackers and told to inspect various 4x4 inch plates and answer questions about the targets after the presentation of the targets. Schieber, et al. (2006), found that the fluorescent target was more likely to attract initial fixations from the participant than the non-fluorescent targets. This suggests that fluorescent colors automatically attract or grab visual attention over non-fluorescent colors. Lahrmann and Madsen (2015) utilized the attention grabbing quality of fluorescent material in their study. Half of the participants in their study were instructed to wear fluorescent yellow jackets while the other half of participants were instructed to wear their normal riding gear during their daily rides. Lahrmann and Madsen (2015) found that the cyclists who wore the fluorescent yellow jackets reported 53% fewer crashes than the bicyclists who were not given a yellow jacket. Cyclists, with the addition of a brightly colored yellow jacket, were able to increase their chances of avoiding collisions. However, another study found that the 30% of bicyclists who actually wear fluorescent colors while cycling, were more likely to avoid collisions injuries [8]. Even motorcyclist can utilize the attention grabbing quality of fluorescent material. Wells et al. (2004) conducted a study assessing factors that contribute to motorcyclists and car collisions in urban environments. Using crash report data, surveys, and interviews with motorcyclists, they found that a motorcyclist was 37% more likely to avoid a collision with a vehicle while wearing fluorescent colors than a motorist not wearing fluorescent colors in urban environments during the day. McIntyre, Gugerty, and Duchowski (2011), used eye tracking technology to compare the attention grabbing qualities of red taillights verses yellow taillights in motor vehicles. Participants, while wearing eye trackers, were instructed to press the spacebar on a keyboard if the cars on the computer monitor pressed on their breaks. They found that searching for red taillights among many yellow taillights decreases reaction time in comparisons to searching for red taillights among red taillights suggesting that the more contrast a target has with its surrounding, the easier and faster it will be found [4]. 3 Method 3.1 Participants Approximately 12 participants (7 males) who are between the ages of 21 and 27 (M=22 years, SD=2 years), have good vision (while wearing corrective lenses, if normally worn), and have normal color vision were eligible to participate in this study. Participants with any self-reported visual impairments were excluded. Four participants were excluded from analysis because of methodological issues. The four participants were excluded because of incomplete or absent eye tracking data, resulting in nine participants with usable data for this experiment. Because the nature of this experiment is a visual search task, participants must be able to demonstrate a base level of visual acuity and be able to discriminate various colors to complete the task. If participants were unable to meet this requirement, they were excluded from the experiment. Participants were recruited through emails and general announcements in various classes at. 3.2 Design This experiment is a 2 (Bicyclist Distance: Near and Far) x 4 (Bicycle Color: Black, White, Yellow, and Blue) within subjects design. Each participant saw all four bicycle colors photographed at the near distance and the far distance. Participants also saw six novel images of similar roadway environments as the images containing a bicyclist that do not contain a bicyclist. These six images were randomly repeated to equal a total of 24 images including the 8 bicycle images. The purpose of presenting the images in random order is to control for order effects. The time it takes each participant to fixate on the bicyclist will be recorded as the main dependent variable. A verbal confirmation of yes or no from the participant will also be recorded, along with a key press on the space bar to indicate if the participant detects a bicyclist. Figure 1: Bicyclist on a black bike from a far distance. Figure 2: Bicyclist on a white bike from a far distance. 2

Figure 3: Bicyclist on a yellow bike from a far distance. Figure 7: Bicyclist present: Yellow Bike (Near distance) Figure 4: Bicyclist on a blue bike from a far distance. Figure 8: Bicyclist present: Blue Bike (Near distance) Figure 5: Bicyclist on a black bike from a nearer distance. Figure 9: Absent Cyclist Figure 6: Bicyclist on a white bike from a nearer distance. Figure 10: Absent Cyclist 3

Gazepoint GP3 Eye-Tracker offering an accuracy of 1 degree of visual angle and a sampling rate of 60 Hz according to the manufacturer. The Gazepoint GP3 Eye-Tracker is a binocular pupil/corneal reflection eye tracker. Figure 11: Absent Cyclist Figure 12: Absent Cyclist Figure 13: Absent Cyclist Figure 14: Absent Cyclist 4 Materials 4.1 Apparatus The images will be presented to participants on a 22 inch Dell monitor with a resolution of 1680 x 1050. Participants gaze will be tracked during each experimental session using a 5 Procedure Each participant will be greeted in the lab and read a script about the study. They will then read an informational letter approved by Institutional Review Board. The participant will be given the opportunity to ask any questions he/she may have about the experiment. Once the participant has finished reading the letter, they will answer demographic questions about their age, gender, or any visual impairments that may skew the results of the experiment. Participants will then be set up on a 22 inch Dell computer and fitted to the Gaze Point eye tracker. To increase the accuracy of the data collected from the eye tracker, the user will be asked to calibrate the eye tracker using an image of nine numbered circles. After calibration, the participant will view images with and without a cyclist. Participants are asked to identify if the image contains a cyclist by pressing the space key on the keyboard and verbally confirming with either a yes or a no. An experimenter recorded the verbal response. Participants will then take a short pen and paper survey concerning what they saw on the computer screen, as well as giving a brief summary of their driving experience administered by an experimenter. Once this is completed, participants will be thanked and dismissed. 6 Results 6.1 Time to Fixation A repeated measure ANOVA with a Greenshouse-Geisser correction determined that the near bicycle color did not significantly differ in reaction time to the bicycle between the four colors (black, white, yellow, and blue) (F (1.919,15.355)=1.336, p=.291). Post hoc test using the Bonferroni correction found that the black bicycle was not significantly different from the yellow (p=.487), white (p=1.00), or blue bicycles (p= 1.00). The blue bicycle was not significantly different than the yellow (p=.863) or white bicycle (p=.562). Lastly, the yellow bicycle was not found to be significantly different that the white bicycle (p=1.00). Due to calibration errors, data from the far bicycles were unobtainable, as all participants fixations missed the stimulus AOI for time to first fixation on the far bicycles. Though the near bicycle colors were not found to be statically different from each other, there is an interesting pattern concerning near bicycle color. The near blue bicyclist had the lowest and best reaction time of all of the colors (M=.895 seconds, SD=.298 seconds) followed by the near black bicycle (M=1.063 seconds, SD=.477 seconds), the near yellow bicycle (M=1.318 seconds, SD=.740), and the near white bicycle (M=1.327, SD=.534), as indicated in Figure 15. 4

Frequency Figure 16: Total time spent on an image. Figure 15: Mean time to first fixation for near bicycle colors. 6.2 Total Time on the Image Total time on the image is a secondary measure for bicycle visibility as participants were instructed to press the spacebar once they found the image. The total time spent on an image indicated reaction time from when the participant was presented with the image to the time when they found the bicyclist. A twoway repeated measures ANOVA was performed to assess the effect of distance and bicycle color on the total time spent on the image. There was not a significant interaction between distance and bicycle color, F (3)=1.677, p=.199. Although we did not find a statistically significant effect, we did find that blue had the fastest reaction time for the near images and black had the fastest reaction time for the far distance. Other bicycle color total times can be found in Table 1. However, we did find an interesting pattern, as the yellow and black bicycles appear to be robust to distance as shown in Figure 16. Near Far M SD M SD Black 2.009 0.491 2.189 0.436 Blue 2.089 0.566 3.905 3.723 Yellow 2.286 1.201 2.284 0.406 White 2.338 1.233 2.716 0.597 6.3 Post Experimental Questionnaire Participants reported that they have been driving for an average of 5.44 years (SD=3.43 years) and they reported driving an average of 5.26 hours per week (SD=4.02 hours) 77.8% of participants consider themselves to be an experience driver while 11.1% consider themselves to be an expert driver and 11.1% consider themselves to be a beginner driver as indicated in Figure 17. A correlation between level of driver experience and reaction time to spotting the yellow bicycle was found to be significant, r(7)= -.682, p=.043. 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 0 11.1% 0% 88.9% 11.1% Beginner Novice Experience Expert Driver Experience Figure 17: What level of experience do you have with driving? The blue bicycle was reported as being the easiest to see by 55.6% of the participants as indicated in Figure 19. 88.9% of participants preferred the blue colored bicycle as shown in Figure 19. Table 1: Means and Standard Deviations total time spent on an image for each bicycle color and distance. 5

No significant reaction was found between distance and bicycle color on the total time spent on a image. This contradicts past research which found that fluorescent material attracts attention faster than non fluorescent material [6];[3]. Although not statistically significant, blue had the fastest reaction time for the near images and black at the fastest reaction time for the far distance according to the group means. The lack of a statistically significant effect is most likely due to a small sample size. Figure 18 : Frequency of participants response to the question Which color bicycle was easiest to see? Figure 19: Frequency of participant s response to the question Which color bicycle did you prefer? 7 Discussion Calibration errors prevented results from being accurately recorded for all of the far bicyclists. When calibrating the participants for the experiment, a problem occurred where each participant could not be accurately calibrated for the right side of the screen. This error caused all participants to miss our far AOIs for the far cyclist positioned on the far right of the screen. Another problem with the far AOIs, was that its AOI size was much smaller than the near AOIs. The smaller target added to the difficulty of getting hits for the far AOIs since the participants were not accurately calibrated. Bicycle colors for the near cyclist did not significantly differ in reaction time between the four colors (black, white, yellow, and blue). In post hoc comparison, none of the bicycle colors differed in reaction time from any of the other bicycle colors. This could be due to the small sample size which offers a low experimental power. However, we did find that the near blue bicyclist had the lowest and best reaction time of all of the colors followed by the black bicycle, the yellow bicycle, and the white bicycle according to group means. Even though this is not a significant effect, it is a promising pattern to expand upon in future studies with a larger sample size. Through further analysis, the yellow and black bicycles appear to be robust to distance. This pattern could suggest regardless of distance, the yellow and black bicycles are found in the same amount of time. This could mean that when it comes to yellow and black colored bicyclist, distance is not a major influencing factor, suggesting that yellow and black colored bicycles could increase bicyclist visibility and therefore safety. The robust to distance pattern of the fluorescent yellow bicycle is supported by past research conducted by Schieber, Willan and Schlorholtz (2006), where it was found that fluorescent colors immediately attracted the driver attention over non-fluorescent colors. Interestingly, the blue bicycle was reported as being the easiest to see out of the four colors and the most preferred by participants even though participants took longer to fixate on the blue bicycles in the image. Blue may have been the preferred color by the participants for reasons unrelated to the experiment (ex. favorite color) and could have introduced bias. Bicycle designers can use this information and increase the amount of blue in their bicycle designs and athletic apparel for cyclists. Designers can apply this information to a variety of popular colors. The more experience with driving the participant reported, the less amount of time they spent on the yellow bicycle images, and therefore the faster they spotted the yellow bicycle. The correlation suggest that driver experience may be related to the shorter reaction times of finding yellow colored bicycles. This could be due to a learned association from experience. Many visibility aids are fluorescent colored and the association that experienced drivers have with seeing visibility aids on the road could help drivers find yellow colored bicyclist faster. Though we had the limitations of a small sample size and calibration errors of the eye tracker, the design of the experiment and hypothesis are good tools for testing how bicycle colors affect visibility of the rider to others and should be expanded upon in future studies in order to influence changes in bicycling safety. This particular study could be replicated with larger sample sizes and more accurate eye tracker calibration. The study could, with increased accuracy, determine the effect of bicycle color in visibility. It is important that cyclists be as safe as possible when riding their bicycles amongst vehicle traffic and pedestrian traffic, as they are highly susceptible to injury if they are not visible enough. 6

8 References [1] Hagel, B. E., Lamy, A., Rizkallah, J. W., Belton, K. L., Jhangri, G. S., Cherry, N., & Rowe, B. H. (2007). The prevalence and reliability of visibility aid and other risk factor data for uninjured cyclists and pedestrians in Edmonton, Alberta, Canada. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 39(2), 284-289. [2] Khorasani-Zavareh, D., Bohm, K., Khankeh, H., Talebian, M.T., Mohammadi, R., Bigdeli, M., & Castren, M. (2014). Why should being visible on the road? A challenge to prevent road traffic injuries among pedestrians in Iran. Journal of injury and violence research, 7(2), 93-94. [3] Lahrmann, H., & Madsen, T. K. O. (2015). The safety impact of a yellow bicycle jacket. Safety Science. [4] McIntyre, S., Gugerty, L., & Duchowski, A. (2012). Brake lamp detection in complex and dynamic environments: Recognizing limitations of visual attention and perception. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 45, 588-599. [5] National Center for Statistics and Analysis. (2017). Bicyclists and other cyclists: 2015 data. (Traffic Safety Facts. Report No. DOT HS 812 382). Washington, DC: National Highway Traffic Safety Administration. [6] Räsänen, M., & Summala, H. (1998). Attention and expectation problems in bicycle car collisions: an in-depth study. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 30(5), 657-666. [7] Schieber, F., Willan, N., & Schlorholtz, B. (2006, October). Fluorescent colored stimuli automatically attract visual attention: An eye movement study. In Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting(Vol. 50, No. 16, pp. 1634-1637). Sage CA: Los Angeles, CA: Sage Publications. [8] Thornley, S. J., Woodward, A., Langley, J. D., Ameratunga, S. N., & Rodgers, A. (2008). Conspicuity and bicycle crashes: preliminary findings of the Taupo Bicycle Study. Injury Prevention, 14(1), 11-18. [9] Wells, S., Mullin, B., Norton, R., Langley, J., Connor, J., Jackson, R., & Lay-Yee, R. (2004). Motorcycle rider conspicuity and crash related injury: case-control study. 7