Qualitative and quantitative pedestrian research in NSW

Similar documents
PEDESTRIAN AND DRIVER KNOWLEDGE OF PRIORITY RULES FOR VARIOUS TYPES OF CROSSINGS J. Hatfield 1, R.F.S. Job 2 & K. Smith 3

TRIAL EVALUATION OF WIDE, AUDIO-TACTILE, CENTRELINE CONFIGURATIONS ON THE NEWELL HIGHWAY

Quantitative study of attitudes, motivations and beliefs related to speeding and speed enforcement.

An evaluation of pedestrian countdown timers in the Sydney CBD

Low Level Cycle Signals used as repeaters of the main traffic signals Appendices

Understanding school road safety

NZ Transport Agency Safer journeys for schools: guidelines for school communities 2

Baseline Survey of New Zealanders' Attitudes and Behaviours towards Cycling in Urban Settings

At each type of conflict location, the risk is affected by certain parameters:

TYPES OF CYCLING. Figure 1: Types of Cycling by Gender (Actual) Figure 2: Types of Cycling by Gender (%) 65% Chi-squared significance test results 65%

Low Level Cycle Signals with an early release Appendices

Reduction of Speed Limit at Approaches to Railway Level Crossings in WA. Main Roads WA. Presenter - Brian Kidd

Draft Railway Crossing Safety Strategy. RAA feedback on the Draft Railway Crossing Safety Strategy

FLETCHER AVENUE PEDESTRIAN AND BICYCLIST BEHAVIOR CHANGE FORMATIVE RESEARCH PROJECT

Life Transitions and Travel Behaviour Study. Job changes and home moves disrupt established commuting patterns

Community Perceptions of Speeding

Determining bicycle infrastructure preferences A case study of Dublin

School Bus Safety in Australia Newman, Shannon L., & Tziotis, Michael, (ARRB Transport Research Ltd).

A Conceptual Approach for Using the UCF Driving Simulator as a Test Bed for High Risk Locations

Frequently asked questions about how the Transport Walkability Index was calculated are answered below.

Glasgow City Council. Evaluation of Glasgow s School Bus Signage Pilot - Final Report

Deaths/injuries in motor vehicle crashes per million hours spent travelling, July 2007 June 2011 (All ages) Mode of travel

THE DEVELOPMENT OF MALAYSIAN HIGHWAY RAIL LEVEL CROSSING SAFETY SYSTEMS: A PROPOSED RESEARCH FRAMEWORK. Siti Zaharah Ishak

Napier City road trauma for Napier City. Road casualties Estimated social cost of crashes* Major road safety issues.

Evaluating the Influence of R3 Treatments on Fishing License Sales in Pennsylvania

Cyclists and red lights a study of behaviour of commuter cyclists in Melbourne

road safety issues 2001 road toll for Gisborne district July 2002 Road user casualties Estimated social cost of crashes*

Rural Economy and Connectivity Committee. Restricted Roads (20mph Speed Limit) (Scotland) Bill: Key Themes Arising from the Online Survey

Pedestrian crossings survey in Europe

Investment in Active Transport Survey

BRIEFING PAPER 29 FINDINGS SERIES. Children s travel to school are we moving in the right direction?

Qualitative research into motivations and barriers to cycling Russell Greig Department of Transport, Western Australia.

Double Pair Comparisons PART III AGE & GENDER. Age and Crash Risk. Subject & control groups Relative risk or rate. Relative Accident Involvement Ratio

Distance between Speed Humps and Pedestrian Crossings: Does it Matter?

Pedestrian and driver encounters, communication and decision strategies Matus Sucha

Paper submitted to the Scottish Transport Studies Group (STSG) April 2004

Using a Mixed-Method Approach to Evaluate the Behavioural Effects of the Cycling City and Towns Programme

TECHNICAL NOTE THROUGH KERBSIDE LANE UTILISATION AT SIGNALISED INTERSECTIONS

Developing a Safer Cycling Strategy for the ACT ACRS Conference August 2012

DOI /HORIZONS.B P23 UDC : (497.11) PEDESTRIAN CROSSING BEHAVIOUR AT UNSIGNALIZED CROSSINGS 1

Trial 3: Interactions Between Autonomous Vehicles and Pedestrians and Cyclists

Wipe Off 5 A Victorian Social Marketing Campaign

MYTH-BUSTING IN MELBOURNE Left-turn Vehicle vs. Pedestrian Crashes At Signalised Intersections

MEMORANDUM. Charlotte Fleetwood, Transportation Planner

Human factors of pedestrian walking and crossing behaviour

2014 QUICK FACTS ILLINOIS CRASH INFORMATION. Illinois Emergency Medical Services for Children February 2016 Edition

2012 QUICK FACTS ILLINOIS CRASH INFORMATION. Illinois Emergency Medical Services for Children September 2014 Edition

Children s risks on their way to school: the example of Tallinn

Pedestrian Safety Campaign

HINDSIGHT SITUATIONAL EXAMPLE. unexpected runway crossing

Pedestrian Countdown at Traffic Signal Junctions (PCaTS) - Road Trial

UNDERSTANDING PEDESTRIAN CROSSING BEHAVIOUR: A CASE STUDY IN SOUTH AFRICA

Appendix A: Crosswalk Policy

NMT SAFE STUDY APPROACH

R J R F. Night-time accidents. A scoping study. Heather Ward +, Nigel Shepherd*, Sandy Robertson +, and Mary Thomas*

People killed and injured per million hours spent travelling, Motorcyclist Cyclist Driver Car / van passenger

Pedestrian and driver encounters, communication and decision strategies

the Ministry of Transport is attributed as the source of the material

A review of 2015 fatal collision statistics as of 31 December 2015

telephone survey EDMONTON AND AREA TRAFFIC SAFETY CULTURE SURVEY: SUMMARY OF KEY FINDINGS 2014

Social marketing can it be used to reduce road traffic collisions among young male Irish drivers?

Lane Area Transportation Safety and Security Plan Vulnerable Users Focus Group

SH1, Karo Drive Wellington Crash Reduction Study

NZ Transport Agency Cycling Rules

EXECUTIVE SUMMARY March 2013

IMPROVING PEDESTRIAN SAFETY AT UNCONTROLLED CROSSINGS. Guidelines for Marked Crosswalks

RESTRICTED ROADS (20 MPH SPEED LIMIT) (SCOTLAND) BILL. 1. Is reducing the speed limit to 20mph the best way of achieving the aims of the Bill?

Contents. Champaign-Urbana Mass Transit District Stop Placement Guidelines

Walking and Cycling Action Plan Summary. A Catalyst for Change The Regional Transport Strategy for the west of Scotland

Kevin Manaugh Department of Geography McGill School of Environment

Safety and Active Transport. Dr. Maureen Carew, Medical Officer of Health Renfrew County and District Health Unit May 30, 2014

Do As I Say Not As I Do: Observed Compliance vs. Stated Understanding of Pedestrian Crossing Laws in Florida

NYC Pedestrian Safety Study & Action Plan. Research, Implementation & Safety Division of Traffic Operations

The 2002 Pedestrian Safety Awareness Campaign

DRIVING ON THE HARD SHOULDER A SAFETY ASSESSMENT ABSTRACT

Children s expectations and beliefs toward the relative safety of riding bicycles at night

briefing notes - road safety issues

DEPARTMENT OF FISHERIES. COMMUNITY PERCEPTIONS RESEARCH May 2013

Bicycle - Motor Vehicle Collisions on Controlled Access Highways in Arizona

Road Safety Partnership

Discerning Pedestrian Accidents in Dhaka-Barisal National Highway in Bangladesh

Clothing for Improving Mine Worker Visibility

SAFETY GUIDE FOR SCHOOL CHILDREN & PARENTS. toronto.ca/visionzeroto #VisionZeroTO

1999 On-Board Sacramento Regional Transit District Survey

Crashes of older Australian riders

Traffic Parameter Methods for Surrogate Safety Comparative Study of Three Non-Intrusive Sensor Technologies

2015 Victorian Road Trauma. Analysis of Fatalities and Serious Injuries. Updated 5 May Page 1 of 28. Commercial in Confidence

This objective implies that all population groups should find walking appealing, and that it is made easier for them to walk more on a daily basis.

We will also cover topics including sharing the road with: Emergency vehicles; Pedestrians; and Animals.

Merseyside Road Safety Partnership s Annual Road Traffic Casualties Report 2015

2003 road trauma for. Wairoa District. Road casualties Estimated social cost of crashes* Major road safety issues WAIROA DISTRICT JULY 2004

Response Road Safety Strategy for New South Wales

Risk on the Road. Pedestrians, Cyclists and Motorcyclists August 2015

Understanding Interactions between Drivers and Pedestrian Features at Signalized Intersections

Every time a driver is distracted,

ROUNDABOUTS IN AUSTRALIA Views of Australian Professionals

ENCOURAGING TAXI DRIVERS TO BEHAVE: GRAFTON BRIDGE TAXI AND BUS LANE TRIAL

DOT HS September Crash Factors in Intersection-Related Crashes: An On-Scene Perspective

Frequently asked questions (FAQ) about a borough-wide 20 mph speed limit

Student Pedestrian Safety Fiddlers Canyon Elementary Community Council 2012

Transcription:

Qualitative and quantitative pedestrian research in NSW Greg Allsopp*, Lyndall Johnson*, David Span #, Mich Preston #, Eliane Morel #, Camden FitzGerald^, Qingjian Jiang^, Doris Lee^ *NSW Centre for Road Safety, Roads and Traffic Authority of NSW #AMR Interactive ^Parsons Brinckerhoff Pty Ltd Introduction In NSW, pedestrian fatalities represent about one fifth of the road toll and as high as a third of all road deaths in metropolitan areas. Between 2002 and 2006, around 70 per cent of pedestrian fatalities occurred in the Sydney, Newcastle and Wollongong conurbation. Previous research commissioned by the Roads and Traffic Authority of NSW (RTA) has highlighted several pedestrian safety issues, including a general belief that the road is for drivers rather than pedestrians, confusion over who has right of way in different situations, pedestrians desire to take the shortest route with no regard for pedestrian facilities, driver intolerance of risky pedestrian behaviours, and the importance of non-verbal communication between drivers and pedestrians (AMR Interactive, 2003; Job and Hatfield, 2002; Woolcott Research, 2004). The current pedestrian study has been designed to include a review of the existing literature, an observational study of pedestrian and driver behaviour and a survey of attitudes and self-reported behaviours regarding pedestrian safety. A literature review of the risk factors for pedestrian crashes was undertaken by the NSW Injury Risk Management Research Centre in 2006, but is as yet unpublished. A pilot observational study of pedestrian and driver behaviour at 10 selected pedestrian facilities in the Sydney metropolitan area has just been completed on behalf of the RTA by Parsons Brinckerhoff Pty Ltd. The results of the pilot observational study will form the basis for a large-scale, comprehensive observational study of pedestrian and driver behaviour at up to 100 selected pedestrian facilities in the Sydney, Newcastle and Wollongong conurbation.

A survey of community attitudes towards pedestrian safety is currently underway and is due to be completed by the end of 2007. This survey is being undertaken on behalf of the RTA by AMR Interactive, and complements the observational study. Previous qualitative research identified a range of issues relating to pedestrian safety, including the behaviour of drivers at pedestrian crossings, risk taking by both drivers and pedestrians, and a road system which is perceived to favour motorists (AMR Interactive, 2003). This current attitudinal research will enable us to further explore community attitudes and behaviours towards pedestrian safety, and to gain a deeper understanding of the factors that contribute to pedestrian crashes. This paper will outline the methodologies utilised in the pilot observational study and the attitudinal survey, and will discuss the preliminary findings that were available at the time of writing. Pilot observational study Aims The main aim of the pilot study was to evaluate alternate data capture methodologies to determine whether they could accurately and efficiently capture and analyse the required data, and would be appropriate for a large scale study. The aim of the large scale study will be to determine if and how variables such as the type of pedestrian facility, the crossing location, the age of the pedestrian and the gender of the pedestrian might contribute to driver and pedestrian behaviour. While the sample sizes were very small in the pilot study, the indicative behavioural findings have been reported in this paper. Methodology The pilot observational study required an enormous amount of data to be captured, such as: Characteristics of the site. Date, time and weather conditions. Age and gender. Characteristics of pedestrian behaviour. Characteristics of driver behaviour. Illegally parked vehicles that might adversely affect sight distances. Characteristics of pedestrian and driver interactions.

Pedestrian crossing behaviour within 50 m on either side of the facility, or the designated location in the absence of a pedestrian facility. Traffic and pedestrian counts at each site. The data were required to be collected on a weekday, outside of school holidays, and over the eight hour period of 8:00 am to 12:00 noon and 3:00 pm to 7:00 pm. The types of pedestrian facilities that were being observed included: Pedestrian crossing (zebra), both mid-block and at an intersection. Pedestrian crossing (zebra) with kerb extensions or refuge elements, both midblock and at an intersection. Pedestrian refuge. Signalised pedestrian crossing, both mid-block and at an intersection. No facilities provided, both mid-block and at an intersection. The behavioural observations were achieved using a mix of video and manual observations. All traffic and pedestrians counts were obtained from video recordings. A critical element of the behavioural observations was the design of the survey instrument. Substantial effort went into its design to facilitate the ease and consistency of data capture. The same survey instrument was used by both the manual and video observers. Manual observations Pedestrian and driver behaviour was recorded by two trained observers at each site. The observers were positioned on opposite sides of the pedestrian facility. Each observer recorded details for pedestrians that would be facing them while crossing the road. This eliminated the possibility of duplication and allowed the observer a better view of the pedestrian s behaviour. As pedestrians walked along the footpath on the other side of the road, the observer would randomly select one person to observe. That person would then be observed until they had completed crossing the road, either at the pedestrian facility or within 50 m on either side. Driver behaviour, parked vehicles, etc were all recorded relative to each pedestrian observed. Video observations The study utilised digital video recorders mounted on three metre poles. Several camera configurations were tested during the study. The video data was then analysed by five observers using the same observation criteria as were utilised by the manual observers. Up to 100 per cent of pedestrians could be observed using the

video footage. However, at sites with very high pedestrian volumes, the data were sampled using the same random method as the manual observations. Results The video method provided an opportunity to capture a greater number of observations, around 73 per cent of the pedestrians counted, as opposed to the manual method which captured 32 per cent. However, counting errors at the Surry Hills site resulted in more pedestrians being observed than were counted. Problems experienced setting up the video equipment at Lidcombe meant only 6 hours of data were captured, also resulting in more pedestrians observed than counted. The pedestrian count at Bondi Beach was under represented due to lost data for the last 2.5 hours of capture. The video equipment appeared unable to capture the level of detail required, especially concerning the age of the pedestrians and behavioural characteristics such as whether the pedestrian looked for vehicles before crossing, or whether the pedestrian waited for vehicles to slow or stop before crossing. Loss of data was also a major issue associated with the video capture method, and was the cause of three of the sites being resurveyed. Difficulties were also encountered with the manual observations. The data collection sheets were necessarily complex due to the amount of data being collected in this study. The manual observers had been briefed and trained prior to data collection. However the method for observing pedestrians was altered at some of the sites to enable all legs of the pedestrian crossing to be captured. The complexity of the data sheets, coupled with the inherent differences between observers, inevitably led to errors in the data. The main issues with the manual data collection were missing data and incorrect coding. A comparison of the video and manual methods is summarised in Table 1. The data analyses have been undertaken as part of the overall testing and evaluation of the research methodology. Given the small number of sites included in the pilot study, the following results are indicative only and not necessarily considered to be representative of behaviours at pedestrian facilities. General trends observed included: Younger people had a greater tendency to cross the road away from pedestrian facilities than older people. Pedestrians would not go out of their way to use refuges or, to a lesser extent, mid-block signalised crossings.

Drivers were more likely to take evasive action at signalised and zebra crossings, whereas pedestrians were more likely to take evasive action at refuges and in locations where there are no facilities. Conflicts were caused by a range of behaviours, eg: o Pedestrians not looking, particularly on zebra crossings. o Pedestrians not waiting for a clear gap at refuges. o Pedestrians running across the road at any location.

Table 1. A comparison of the video and manual data collection methods, including the number of pedestrians observed and the main data issues encountered. Site Total pedestrians counted Manual observations 1 Lidcombe 248 250 101% 248 100% 2 Burwood 321 210 65% Video observations Manual data issues Video data issues Resurvey required count and yes observational data incomplete incorrect location code inconsistent recording of people not using the facility count data lost 3 Fairfield 1,660 386 23% 4 Merrylands 4,036 3,809 94% 5 Liverpool 2,044 827 40% count data lost yes 6 Guilford 2,252 2,252 100% 7 Mona Vale 3,648 671 18% 8 Kensington 6,072 2,734 45% 9 Surry Hills 523 598 114% 10 Bondi Beach 597 340 57% Total 21,401 2,684 32% 9,641 73% inconsistent and poor recording of people not using the facility incorrect location code suspected traffic signal fault during first survey poor weather during first survey count data lost yes yes yes Extrapolate count from number of manual observations

Attitudinal study Aims The aims of the attitudinal study were to: Determine general community attitudes towards pedestrian safety. Identify major themes in pedestrian safety. Identify community attitudes to specific pedestrian safety issues. Identify the key factors influencing pedestrian and driver behaviours relating to pedestrian safety, including core beliefs, attitudes, prior behaviours and experiences relating to drivers and pedestrians. Methodology The attitudinal study incorporated both qualitative and quantitative components of research. The qualitative component comprised 10 focus groups with residents from Sydney, Wollongong, Newcastle and the Central Coast. The focus groups utilised projective techniques, which can reveal deeply held attitudes and motivations that are often not verbalised when people are questioned directly. The groups were predominantly distinguished by age, and included a mixed group of 17-25 year olds, a male only group of 17-25 year olds, mixed groups of 26-39 year olds, 40-59 year olds, 60+, 70+, and parents aged 26-59. The focus groups identified the main themes, issues, misconceptions, common terminology, etc, relating to pedestrian safety. These factors contributed to the design of the telephone questionnaire, which comprised the quantitative component of the study. Prior to implementation, the telephone questionnaire was question-tested to ensure the intent of each question was clear. The questionnaire was also pilot tested on a sample of 200 people to test the statistical validity of the instrument. The survey was then conducted with 1000 residents from Sydney, Wollongong, Newcastle and the Central Coast. Results At the time of writing this paper, the quantitative component of the attitudinal study had not been completed. Therefore, only the key findings from the focus groups will be reported and discussed. A number of themes and factors around pedestrian and driver behaviour were raised in the groups. Generally, the focus group participants thought pedestrian safety was becoming more important as traffic volumes were increasing. Parents were

particularly aware of pedestrian safety issues, especially when walking with their children. Older people, over 60 years, also appeared more conscious of pedestrian safety issues, whilst young people, 17-25 years, appeared to be the least likely to think about pedestrian safety. Location affected participants attitudes, with pedestrians more conscious of pedestrian safety in suburban or semi-rural areas, while drivers appeared more concerned about pedestrian safety in higher density urban areas. Participants who had been involved in, or witnessed, an accident were also more likely to be concerned about pedestrian safety. Minor accidents or incidents appeared to have a short term effect on behaviour, while major accidents had a more profound, long term effect. Drivers mentioned several pedestrian behaviours that were likely to intimidate them, eg: Pedestrians stepping out without looking. Jaywalking, especially if forced to slow down for the pedestrian. Pedestrians who are distracted and not paying attention to the road. Alcohol-affected pedestrians. Elderly pedestrians who may be quite slow. Pedestrians who stand in the middle of the road waiting to cross. Unpredictable behaviour from children, which frightens most drivers. Pedestrians also mentioned driver behaviours that were likely to intimidate them, eg: Drivers who sped up when approaching pedestrian crossings to avoid stopping. Intimidating behaviour in an attempt to hasten the pedestrian off the road. Drivers who are distracted and not paying attention to the road. I own the road attitude exhibited by some drivers. Participants who exhibited intimidating behaviours as a pedestrian were not necessarily tolerant, as a driver, of those behaviours in other pedestrians, and vice versa. Drivers tended to believe they obeyed the rules most of the time. When rules were broken, such as speeding on high speed roads and running red lights, they were not seen as a significant threat to pedestrian safety as they would not expect pedestrians to be crossing in those situations. Driver behaviour appeared to be moderated by the risk of being caught and penalised for breaking the rules, as well as a perception of the car as a killing machine. On the other hand, many pedestrians admitted to frequently breaking the rules. There was an almost universal belief that they would not be penalised. Those

pedestrians who took risks saw their behaviour as risk management rather than risk taking, as they felt in control, able to weigh up the situation and could justify their behaviour, eg they were only risking their own safety, not anyone else s. Pedestrian behaviour appeared to be motivated by a combination of impatience, laziness or convenience, and a sense of invincibility. Figure 1 illustrates the key factors which affect pedestrian risk-taking behaviour. Scenario - distance and speed of approaching car - driver awareness (eye contact) Location and conditions - time of day - weather conditions (risk of slipping) - visibility (dark clothing) - who else is around Instinctive risk management Volume of traffic and pedestrians - fewer cars, pedestrians more likely to jaywalk - herd mentality - I ll often walk if I can be between other pedestrians Risk-taking behaviour Invincibility Rushing - running for the bus -impatience - less likely to take risks if have more time Inattention - unaware and oblivious to risks - distracted, eg phone, ipod - I don t even think about the danger, I ll just step out Figure 1. Key factors that affect the level of pedestrian risk-taking behaviour. Other factors were also found to influence risk taking behaviour, such as age, gender, and journey type. Younger participants generally felt more confident in their abilities, both as a driver and pedestrian, and did not perceive their behaviour as risktaking. As the participants aged, their behaviour generally became more moderate and their awareness of risk increased (see Figure 2). Gender differences were more marked in the younger age groups, with 17-25 year old males acknowledging riskier behaviour more than females the same age. Gender differences appeared to diminish as age increased. However, among participants in the groups over 60 years, women seemed to place less emphasis on the rules than their male counterparts. When journeys were routine and familiar, both drivers and pedestrians were more likely to acknowledge complacency and higher risk-taking behaviour, whereas they were more cautious on unfamiliar routes. However, when pedestrian journeys

included children, a strong compliance with the rules was indicated, both to protect the child and to set a good example. Risk-taking with children clearly came across as socially unacceptable. 17-25 26-39 40-59 60-70 70+ Parents likely to behave very differently when with children - invincible, sure of fitness and ability - no sense of actual risk (fearless) - always in a hurry, no time - becoming less reckless, more instinctive - becoming more aware of risks - risks only taken when necessary - slower, less able to dart across road - less rushed - not reckless, more sensible -slow - fragile, greater awareness of risk to self - less confident Figure 2. Risk-taking characteristics of different age groups. Generational differences - walked more than young people do today, ie more experience - greater belief in following the rules Discussion The preliminary evaluation of the data collection methods for the observational study indicated clear strengths and weaknesses between the video and manual methods. The in-situ nature of the manual method appeared to provide greater context for the observations and was generally able to capture more detail in both descriptive, eg age and gender, and behavioural observations. However, the possible number of observations is compromised when large numbers of variables need to be recorded, as in this study. This suggests that manual methods may be more suited to pedestrian facilities and sites that experience lower volumes of pedestrian traffic. Inversely, the video method can provide a significantly larger number of observations, but the quality of the video footage may inhibit the level of descriptive and behavioural detail able to be captured. We suspect that sophisticated camera deployment will increase the quality of the video footage, rendering it a satisfactory data collection tool, especially at complex and high volume pedestrian facilities. On preliminary evaluation, the results of the observational study suggest that, rather than committing to either video or manual collection, a better approach may be sitespecific deployment that utilises the strengths of each method while attempting to minimise their weaknesses. However, care would need to be exercised when

analysing the data to reduce any bias that might be introduced by the different methods. Regarding the behavioural data, the indicative trends identified in the observational study were borne out to some extent by the preliminary findings of the attitudinal study, such as the tendency for younger people to engage in potentially risky behaviour and the reluctance for pedestrians to go out of their way to use pedestrian facilities. However, the observational study suggests pedestrian behaviour alone is the cause of conflicts. This attribution of cause is probably an artefact of the data collection and analysis methods, rather than real cause and effect, as some participants in the focus groups acknowledged that, as a driver, they will break the rules at times. There is some evidence to suggest both drivers and pedestrians understand they have a joint responsibility when using the roads, for example the higher likelihood for drivers to take evasive action when pedestrians have the right of way, and for pedestrians to take evasive action when using refuges or in areas where there is no pedestrian facility, ie when they do not have right of way. The general attitudes and behaviours identified in the focus groups showed some consistency with the 2003 study, eg general belief that the road favours drivers over pedestrians, pedestrians reluctance, at times, to go out of their way to use pedestrian facilities, drivers intolerance for risky pedestrian behaviours and the importance of non-verbal communication between drivers and pedestrians. However, some attitudes appeared to have gained prominence in the recent study, such as a greater awareness of the risk of distractions in both drivers and pedestrians, particularly related to the use of ipods and mobile phones. The focus groups were extremely useful in helping to crystallise general attitudes and behaviours in the community, and to identify the language commonly used by people to describe pedestrian facilities and other aspects of pedestrian safety. It has also generated a number of themes and factors around pedestrian safety, and hypotheses about risk-taking, which will be addressed in the telephone survey. Conclusion While people s attitudes and behaviours are very individual, the current observational and attitudinal studies allow us to identify general characteristics within the population. Knowledge of these characteristics, in turn, allows us to develop policies and strategies that target the particular behaviours that cause the most concern in pedestrian safety and that, when addressed, may achieve the greatest gains for the community.

References AMR Interactive (2003). Survey of Community Attitudes on Pedestrian Safety. Prepared for the Roads and Traffic Authority of NSW. Job RFS, Hatfield J. (2002) Observational study of older pedestrian behaviour at various pedestrian facilities. Prepared for the Roads and Traffic Authority of NSW. Woolcott Research (2004). An Exploration of Drink Walking Amongst Young Males. Prepared for the Roads and Traffic Authority of NSW.