Key Words: Attraction, Color Cue, and Wavelength. Introduction

Similar documents
SPECIES RICHNESS IN THREE OCEANFLOOR HABITATS IN BERMUDA BAYS

SCHOOLING BEHAVIOR OF HAEMULON SPP. IN BERMUDA REEFS AND SEAGRASS BEDS

OIMB GK12 CURRICULUM. LIGHT IN THE DEEP SEA Adapted from NOAA s All That Glitters

Conditioned Alarm Behavior in Fathead Minnows (Pimephales promelas) and Test Their Ability

Evidence for predator-induced behavioral plasticity of juvenile red king crab (Paralithodes camtschaticus)

DOMINICA REEF FISH STATUS 2002: An Assessment of the Abundance and Species Composition of Dominican Reef Fishes. ITME Research Reports Number 13

Light in the Deep Sea Teacher Materials

THE REFEREEING IN BASKETBALL- TRENDS AND OPTIMIZATION STRATEGIES OF THE TRAINING AND PERFORMANCE OF REFEREES IN A DIVISION

EFFECTS OF PREDATION ON THE BEHAVIOR OF GAMMARUS MINUS

Causes of Tiger (Panthera tigris) Population Decline, and Potential Consequences if the Decline Continues

Tracking Juvenile Summer Flounder

Oregon Hatchery Research Center January 2014 David L. G. Noakes, Professor & Director

Annual Measurement of Solar UVB at a Reef Site Using a Polyphenylene Oxide Dosimeter

Frequency of Percnon gibbesi in relation to. Diadema antillarum at Rodney s Rock

LINKING THE TAIWAN FISH DATABASE TO THE GLOBAL DATABASE

University of Miami Rosenstiel School of Marine and Atmospheric Science. Billfish Research Program

Factors Affecting Phonotaxis in Male House Crickets, Acheta domesticus

Domain (island) wide estimates of mutton snapper (Lutjanus analis) abundance for three US Caribbean Islands based on habitat-derived densities

Summary of Research within Lamlash Bay No-Take Zone - Science report for COAST July

Key words: community similarity; coral patch reef; Enewetak; reeffish; species diversity; Virgin Islands.

STUDENT ACTIVITY: THE LIFE AND TIMES OF A FISH

Camouflage for Life: Measuring the Adaptive Value of Color

Green Sturgeon Feeding Observations in Humboldt Bay, California

8 TH MEETING DOCUMENT BYC-08 INF-A

STUDY PERFORMANCE REPORT

Hide and Seek. Adapted from: Activity 38 Hide and Seek Living in Water. The National Aquarium in Baltimore, 1997.

Cichlids of East Africa A Model of Vertebrate Radiation. ww.waveformenergetics.com

Jellyfish as a predator and prey of fishes: underwater observations and rearing experiments

[Barve, 4(7): July, 2015] ISSN: (I2OR), Publication Impact Factor: 3.785

New information regarding the impact of fisheries on other components of the ecosystem

Preferential habitat and spatial distribution of 3. species of sicydiine gobies (Genus Sicydium) based. on substrate and flow rate within the

DIURNAL VARIATIONS IN THE AMOUNT OF DIS- SOLVED OXYGEN, ALKALINITY, AND FREE AMMONIA IN CERTAIN FISH PONDS AT FAIRPORT, (IOWA).

Habitat selection during settlement of three Caribbean coral reef fishes: Indications for directed settlement to seagrass beds and mangroves

NBA TEAM SYNERGY RESEARCH REPORT 1

SC European Union 2013 Annual Report. Ad Corten

Catch per unit effort of coastal prawn trammel net fishery in Izmir Bay, Aegean Sea

Factors influencing production

Integrating basic and applied ecology using paired artificial natural reef systems.

CATCH A WAVE June 4-8, 2018

Comparison of Coral Diversity Across Three Reef Habitats

Caddisfly Larvae Visual System: Response to Light

Introduction. o 2. ! "#$ % & ' (" 4 Watt/m 2. Major

Our Council s Own: Oceanography Cadette/Senior/Ambassador Badge

LIFE CYCLE AND COMMUNITY STRUCTURE OF CADDISFLIES (INSECTA:TRICHOPTERA) IN THE NAVASOTA RIVER, TEXAS.

b

Zooplankton Migration Patterns at Scotton Landing: Behavioral Adaptations written by Lauren Zodl, University of Delaware

An experimental study of internal wave generation through evanescent regions

Project Updates January 2015

Walk - Run Activity --An S and P Wave Travel Time Simulation ( S minus P Earthquake Location Method)

Palythoa Abundance and Coverage in Relation to Depth

A It is halved. B It is doubled. C It is quadrupled. D It remains the same.

Comparative growth of pinfish in field mesocosms across marsh, oyster, and soft-bottom habitat types in a Mississippi estuary

Notebooks or journals for drawing and taking notes

The Effects of Body Mass and Water Submersion on Decomposition Rates in Mice and Rats

The Life History of Triturus v. vittatus (Urodela) in Various Habitats

Marine Conservation and Research Expedition Training, Examination and Certification Summary

PSY 2364 Animal Communication. Midterm. Properties of light. Electromagnetic spectrum. Properties of light. Properties of light 10/4/2017

BEFORE YOU OPEN ANY FILES:

Length-Weight Relationship and Condition Factor of Catla catla in Chhirpani Reservoir, Chhattisgarh, India

SC-02-JM-12. Report of the 5 th workshop on diagnosis of the status of the South Pacific Jack mackerel fishery in Peru SNP Scientific Committee

Sea urchin density along a depth gradient at Rodney's Rock and Champagne Bay, Dominica

Energy, Temperature and Heat

Legendre et al Appendices and Supplements, p. 1

b

A Note on Consumer Preference of Smoked Salmon Colour.

Monitoring of sea trout post-smolts, 2012

Fast Tracking the Development of Environmental- Friendly Fishing Methods

Print Article Close Window

Project No: R Visual Lures for Possums. Malcolm Thomas and Fraser Maddigan. Pest Control Research Ltd PO Box 7223 Christchurch New Zealand

Coastal areas have become increasingly under threat in recent years. Climate change is having a huge effect on coastal areas, making them much more

Centre for Transport Studies

Marine Ecosystems. Aquatic Ecosystems Section 2

THE PRIMARY SOURCES OF SHIP NOISE OBSERVED ON THE BOTTOM OF SEA KAROL LISTEWNIK

How to Hide in the Ocean

The stock of blue whiting has been surveyed for the last three years during the spawning period using the research vessel cg. 0.

The Optimal Downhill Slope for Acute Overspeed Running

COASTAL UPWELLING - MONTEREY BAY CALIFORNIA (modified from The Maury Project, AMS)

How does atmospheric pressure vary? Measuring atmospheric pressure at different altitudes above sea level

Inquiry Investigation: Factors Affecting Photosynthesis

Oceanic Society Reef Research Team: Nicole Crane, Avigdor Abelson, Peter Nelson, Giacomo Bernardi, Michelle Paddack, Kate Crosman

Lab Report Outline the Bones of the Story

What s UP in the. Pacific Ocean? Learning Objectives

Cambridge International Examinations Cambridge International Advanced Subsidiary and Advanced Level

A Case Study of Leadership in Women s Intercollegiate Softball. By: DIANE L. GILL and JEAN L. PERRY

Extract from the project Dynamic Mapping of North Sea Spawning - the KINO Report 2016 Statoil contract no

I. Monitoring and Adaptive Management Approach. Potential Effects, Monitoring Studies & Mitigation

A Computational Assessment of Gas Jets in a Bubbly Co-Flow 1

Schooling Behavior of Thayeria. obliqua and Pristella maxillaris

Role of learning in mesh penetration behaviour of haddock (Melanogrammus aeglefinus)

Atsuko YAMAGUCHI. Since the catches of these fish decrease as the waters, including those around western Kyushu and

SOCIETAL GOALS TO DETERMINE ECOSYSTEM HEALTH: A FISHERIES CASE STUDY IN GALVESTON BAY SYSTEM, TEXAS

Reef Check Australia. Magnetic Island Season Report 2016

WELCOME! I am so excited to be with you today!!! HOW TO DIFFERENTIATE COMPREHENSION INSTRUCTION FOR STRUGGLING STUDENTS SESSION 2

Coefficients of Restitution of Balls Used in Team Sports for the Visually Impaired

Acoustic and Visual Survey of Cetaceans at Palmyra Atoll

Protect Our Reefs Grant Interim Report (October 1, 2008 March 31, 2009) Principal investigators: Donald C. Behringer and Mark J.

FYG Backing for Work Zone Signs

The Population Density of Diadema antillarum at Champagne Reef at Scott s Head Soufriere Marine Reserve

BASELINE SURVEY, VISUAL - SITE SPECIFIC

The Hague, Kingdom of the Netherlands October 2016 SC The European Union Annual report

Transcription:

COLOR CUE AND MOVEMENT ATTRACTION OF BERMUDA BREAM DIPLODUS-BERMUDENSIS 1 Jason Silva 2, Biology Department,, 950 Main Street, Worcester, Ma 01610 (jmoreira@clarku.edu) Abstract Bermuda bream (Diplodus bermudensis) were tested to analyze attraction to color cues. Due to their lack of interest in static colors in a preliminary study, motion was introduced. The hypothesis is that Bermuda bream are attracted to orange and blue colors, which are frequently seen in their habitat. Bream were attracted most to the color orange, but seemed to prefer a variety of colors to any single color. With the knowledge acquired from the color cue test it can be applied to other species. Key Words: Attraction, Color Cue, and Wavelength Introduction While snorkeling in coral reefs, I observed that fish follow people who had a variety of colors on their bathing suits. Fish perceive colors differently from humans (Marshall et al.2003, and Losey et al.2003). Color is the wavelength of light. Fish in coral reefs typically display colors from 300-800 nm (Longley 1916, lythgoe, 1966, Marshall et al. 2003), a range that includes colors not seen by the human eye. The human eye can see colors between 390-780 nm. Work has been done on fish s ability to perceive the range wavelengths, but their attraction to any particular wavelength has not been investigated extensively. The coral reefs in Bermuda exhibit a wide range of colors. The purpose of this study is to observe whether fish can distinguish between different color cues and if so what color is most attractive to them. Materials and Methods Study Organism: Bermuda bream (Diplodus bermudensis) was chosen for this study for its abundance and lack of timidity. It is a benthopelagic fish endemic to the waters of Bermuda. Bream have a silver body with a black dot on their tail and grow to about 30.0 cm in length.

Figure 1. Bermuda Bream in tobacco bay following the orange color cue. Study Site: This study was conducted in Tobacco Bay, Bermuda (32 N, 64 W) during October 10 th to 12 th, 2012. The average water temperature for Bermuda in October is 25 F. The air temperature was in the low 30s high 20s during the experiment. It also rained during the three days. All the trials were conducted during low. The three sites at Tobacco Bay were chosen based on their high density of Bermuda bream. The experiment consisted of two different tests: the stand-still-color cue and the motion color cue. Stand Still Color Cue.The stand still color cue was executed by using a secchi disk and tying different colored, plastic-cage lids to a rope that was lowered into the water. The secchi disk has a marked cord for attachment of colored attractants (see supplement 1 for image). The distance between the lids varied with the water level; lids were kept as evenly distributed from each other as possible. Five colors of lids were used (purple, orange, blue, green, and pink), arranged in random order along the line. Only 3 colors were tied at each trial. The colors were either observed or video recorded for 5 minutes. There were a total of 5 trials; each color was tested 3 times. The trial sites were in open water as well as near the corals (Figure 2). Color Cue and Movement. Four colored fabrics (blue, green, orange, and dark blue) were set up similarly to the stand still trial to determine whether fabric makes a difference in static color attraction. The fabric color chosen was based on colors that seemed to interest the Bermuda Bream during preliminary observations as well as which colors were available. Wearing the fabric in speedo form was found the best way to test for motion and color concisely. The researcher swam backwards while recording Journal of Marine Ecology @ Volume 1: Issue 1 Page 62

the fish following. This motion of swimming was used during every trial. Four areas were chosen as swim sites (Figure2). The fish were counted as attracted if they followed the researcher for ~20 seconds. The size of the school was noted as well the number of fish that followed from the school. This was done to calculate the fraction of the fish from the school that were attracted to the cue, to account for variation in school sizes. Due to visibility and roughness of water only 4 trials were conducted for each of the speedo colors along with the multicolored shorts. It is important to note that during all trials, the experimenter swam through the school of fish to ensure they saw the colors. Figure 2. A modified map by Google Earth depicting the study site, Tobacco Bay. This map shows the location of high-density schools of Bermuda Bream, the areas the researcher swam, as well as the sites for the stand still color cue experiments. Results and Discussion Bermuda bream were not attracted to any particular color when in the colored object was not in motion (Figure 3). In order to see if the fish were not attracted to the plasticcage lids or the colors, fabric was used. The results were similar. None of the fish were attracted to colors. However, fish were more responsive to the combination of color and movement (Figure 4). The bream showed almost a 20% higher attraction for multicolored fabric than for any single color. Orange was individually the most attracted color for the Bermuda Bream with 68.75% of the schools attracted to them. Journal of Marine Ecology @ Volume 1: Issue 1 Page 63

2.5 2 Trial 1 Trial 2 Trial 3 Number of fish 1.5 1 0.5 0 Purple Orange Pink Blue Green Colors Figure 3. Stand still color cue. This graph shows the results for the three trials of stand still color attraction. The graph shows that the fish weren t attracted to the color cues only. Two fish approached orange once during the second trial. Mean Percentage (%) 100 90 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 0 A Multicolored Shorts A, B A,B B C Orange Blue Green Dark Blue Color Figure 4.Percentage of schools following color and motion cues. This graphs shows the average percentage of the individuals from the school that followed the color cues. It shows that they were attracted more the multicolored shorts than individual color cues. It also shows the significant difference between the average mean percentages of fish attracted. The groups are divided to A, B, C and each of the groups is significantly different from one another according to the Tukey-Kramer test. Journal of Marine Ecology @ Volume 1: Issue 1 Page 64

After ANOVA indicated that significant differences existed among the mean percentages (F 4,14 =15.25, p=0.0001), Tukey-Kramer tests were conducted in order to identify the significant differences among the colors (Figure 4). The tests revealed that colors could be separated into three groups. Group A consisted of multicolored fabric, orange, and blue. Group B consisted of orange, blue, and green. Group C consisted of dark blue. This allows us to see the gaps in the data and where further research is needed. More trials would be needed to see if there is significant difference between multicolored shorts, orange, blue, and green. Color is wavelength of light. Since it is impossible to observe what fish actually see. Fish could be able to distinguish more clearly between wavelengths. Colors can be matched to the spectrum from short wavelengths (purple and blue) to long wavelengths (red and orange). The multicolored shorts were made up of many colors but the primarily red.. Comparing color to their location on the spectrum shows evidence that Bermuda bream are attracted to colors around 540 nm and higher. It allows us to speculate that increase in attraction correlates with increase in wavelength the longer the wavelength the more attracted the fish are to it. Schooling behavior might be a factor that influenced the high numbers of fish following the color cue (Hiro-Sata1994). If one individual starts following the color the other might follow out of instinct to stay together and not interest of color or motion. Future Research An area for future research could be looking at different color combinations to see which one attracts them most. The colorful shorts attracted the most bream, and they were composed primarily of red, blue, and white. It would be interesting to see what combination they are most attracted to and how those colors tie into their ecosystem. Also, using wavelengths to choose color combination would be of interest. Combining long and short wave lengths, as well as attraction of wavelengths not seen by the human eye (e.g., infrared and ultra violet) would also be of interest. Literature Cited Hiro-Sato, N. 1994. Self-organizing Dynamic Model of Fish Schooling. Journal of Theoretical Biology, Volume 171, Issue 2, 21 November 1994, Pages 123 136 http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/jtbi.1994.1218 Levine, J. S., and Edward F. MacNichol, Jr. "Color vision in fishes." Scientific American 246 (1982): 140+. Academic OneFile. Web. 21 Oct. 2012. Journal of Marine Ecology @ Volume 1: Issue 1 Page 65

Losey, W. N. McFarland, E. R. Loew, J. P. Zamzow, P. A. Nelson, N. J. Marshall, and W. L. Montgomery (2003) Visual Biology of Hawaiian Coral Reef Fishes. I. Ocular Transmission and Visual Pigments. Copeia: September 2003, Vol. 2003, No. 3, pp. 433-454. Longley, W. H. 1916a. Observations upon tropical fishes and inferences from their adaptive coloration. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci 2:733 737. Lythgoe, J. N. 1966. Visual pigments and underwater vision, p. 375 391. In: Light as an ecological factor. R. Bainbridge, G. C. Evans, and O. Rackham (eds.). Blackwell, Oxford. Masuda, R., Kohji T., and Yoh Y. "School for learning: sharing and transmission of feeding information in jack mackerel Trachurus japonicus juveniles." Fisheries Science 78.2 (2012): 269+. Academic OneFile. Web. 21 Oct. 2012. Marshall, N. J. K. Jennings, W. N. McFarland, E. R. Loew, G. S. Losey, and W. L. Montgomery (2003) Visual Biology of Hawaiian Coral Reef Fishes. II. Colors of Hawaiian Coral Reef Fish. Copeia: September 2003, Vol. 2003, No. 3, pp. 455-466. Marshall, J., Jennings, K., W. N. McFarland, E. R. Loew, G. S. Losey, and W. L. Montgomery (2003) Visual Biology of Hawaiian Coral Reef Fishes. III. Environmental Light and an Integrated Approach to the Ecology of Reef Fish Vision. Copeia: September 2003, Vol. 2003, No. 3, pp. 467-480. Journal of Marine Ecology @ Volume 1: Issue 1 Page 66