Individual differences in bait-fishing by the Green-backed Heron Ardeola striata associated with territory quality

Similar documents
Pest animal control. Guiding principles for community groups and landowners

Largemouth Bass. Max 30 inches Average 4-12 inches EATS

Relative Size Selectivity of Trap Nets for Eight Species of Fish'

Final report on fish diet and stomach analyses

Improving post-stocking survival of hatchery reared threatened fish species

All Ireland Daubenton s Bat Waterway Survey. All-Ireland Bat Monitoring Programme is funded by

Fish Survey of Arctic Lake (ID # ), Scott County, Minnesota in 2012

B Buckmoth. Look quickly or you ll miss the buckmoth! These bold black and white moths fly quickly and for only a few weeks in the fall.

Life Cycles: Egg, Nymph, Adult!

Revisiting the Hot Hand Theory with Free Throw Data in a Multivariate Framework

DEVONVALE GOLF & WINE ESTATE COURSE REPORT February 2017

The fishery for jack mackerel in the Eastern Central Pacific by European trawlers in 2008 and 2009

STUDY PERFORMANCE REPORT

CHECKS AND BALANCES. OVERVIEW Students become managers of a herd of animals in a paper-pencil, discussionbased

(80) PHOTOGRAPHIC STUDIES OF SOME LESS FAMILIAR BIRDS. LESSER KESTREL. (Plates 17-19). WITH A PHOTOGRAPH OF THE KESTREL, BY G. K.

Handbook for the identification of yellowfin and bigeye tunas in fresh, but less than ideal condition

Students use wildlife survey study techniques to discover the cause of the drop in a wood duck population.

Blue cod 5 (BCO5) pot mesh size review

Macroinvertabrate and Water Quality Survey of Smelt Brook

A Traditional Method of Hunting Dusky Sengis in Southern Malawi

WHAT CAN WE LEARN FROM COMPETITION ANALYSIS AT THE 1999 PAN PACIFIC SWIMMING CHAMPIONSHIPS?

D. Clifton-Dey M. Walsingham January 1995.

PLAY STRUCTURE ACTIVITIES. Play With Me

FAO SPECIES IDENTIFICATION SHEETS SYNODONTIDAE. Lizardfishes

Countermeasures against Alien Fishes (Largemouth Bass and Bluegill) in Lake Biwa

Which fish is for which state?

Golden Valley Little League Evaluation Stations Outline

Agriculture Zone Winter Replicate Count 2007/08

Gator Hole Graphics. There is usually a six-month period

The spectacular upper pitcher of Nepenthes macfarlanei

Levels of the Savannah. Guide Book

Newsletter Winter 2017

Surf Survey Summary Report

272 Short-eared Owl. SHORT-EARED OWL (Asio flammeus) SEXING IDENTIFICATION

ALTITUDE TRAINING FOR IMPROVING SWIMMING PERFORMANCE AT SEA LEVEL. MITSUMASA MIYASHITA, YOSHITERU MUTOH and YOSHIHARU YAMAMOTO.

Flint River Assessment Appendix

Background of this study

Bullseye Snakehead in Florida; FWCC s Approach to a Non-Native Fish. Kelly Gestring Non-Native Fish and Aquatic Wildlife Coordinator

Cedar Lake Comprehensive Survey Report Steve Hogler and Steve Surendonk WDNR-Mishicot

Apogon abrogramma Fraser and Lachner, 1985

Fish Species. Dorado or Golden Dorado- Classification:

Our foundation introduce Nature and conservation in Lake Izunuma Uchinuma.

A Stream In A Bucket An introduction to aquatic macroinvertebrates and other stream life.


The Round Goby Botulism Connection. Renea A. Ruffing Graduate Research Assistant Penn State University

The Dartboard. Anyone interested in doing some programming could also find a lot of things to do here.

THE BIOLOGY OF THE PRAWN, PALAEMON

Articles & Stories for Notetaking & Summaries

Comparison of Documents Norton: FWS-WDC. 1. What is the Porcupine caribou herd's historic calving range?

PROJECT Nº 96/005: SIZE SELECTIVITY AND RELATIVE FISHING POWER OF BALTIC COD GILL-NETS

UMUKARIKARI AERIAL BOVINE TB CONTROL OPERATION

Point Rating: 3 Size: 10 mm Description: Caddisfly larvae are builders. They make sticky silk from their spit and use it to spin webs for trapping

Atsuko YAMAGUCHI. Since the catches of these fish decrease as the waters, including those around western Kyushu and

LAKE DIANE Hillsdale County (T8-9S, R3W, Sections 34, 3, 4) Surveyed May Jeffrey J. Braunscheidel

STUDY PERFORMANCE REPORT

How to succeed fishing the Ilkley Angling Association (IAA) stretch of the River Wharfe by David Martin, IAA Secretary

TRENDS IN PARTICIPATION RATES FOR WILDLIFE-ASSOCIATED RECREATION BY RACE/ETHNICITY AND GENDER:

Pond Life. Water Boatmen and Pond Skaters. Frogs and Toads

Changes to Traffic Circulation with Centrerunning

Township of Muskoka Lakes Bear Hazard Assessment January 2006

The impact of the grey squirrel as an invasive species on red squirrel populations. Deborah Brady

DIVING IN! Quantifying the underwater behavior of Christmas Shearwaters on Kure Atoll ILANA NIMZ MARS 6040: SEABIRD ECOLOGY

Application of a New Method for Monitoring Lake Trout Abundance in Yukon: Summer Profundal Index Netting (SPIN)

Dauphin Lake Fishery. Status of Walleye Stocks and Conservation Measures

Identifying Moths in Traps for Sweet Corn Pests

The Great Lakes: An Environmental History

A FISHING BAN ON GRENADIER POND

What Fishes Need. Ontario Ministry of Natural Resources, Fishways curriculum. Best Time: Before Go Fish, also as a follow-up to Go Fish

Grouse in the Bavarian Alps: Status, threats and conservation Example: Black grouse in the Bavarian Alps

SCIENTIFIC COMMITTEE SEVENTH REGULAR SESSION August 2011 Pohnpei, Federated States of Micronesia

Species Identification of small juvenile tunas caught in surface fisheries in the Phili... 1/13 ページ

FROM THE OFFICE. Well fellow fisho s, another Month down & winter is almost over, with Greenwell Point just over a Month away.

Coronis Fritillary Field Identification: Reference Guide

What is an insect? An insect is an invertebrate (an animal with no spine) that has three pairs of legs and three

8 TH MEETING DOCUMENT BYC-08 INF-A

Competitive Performance of Elite Olympic-Distance Triathletes: Reliability and Smallest Worthwhile Enhancement

APPENDIX 3. Distribution Maps of Fish Species

Project Updates January 2015

Paulding Soil & Water Conservation District Fish Sale Guide

Foraging: Life as a Hunter-Gatherer

Opleiding Informatica

Cycles. Grand supercycle: multi-century. Supercycle: multi-decade (about years)

HAZARD ANALYSIS CRITICAL CONTROL POINT (HACCP) FOR SEAFOOD SAFETY AND PREVENTING THE MOVEMENT OF AQUATIC INVASIVE SPECIES

FAO SPECIES IDENTIFICATION SHEETS. FAMILY: LETHRINIDAE FISHING AREA 51 (W. Indian Ocean) Lethrinus conchyliatus (Smith, 1959)

Developing an Easily Used Bait Monitoring Method for Oriental Fruit Moth in Mating Disruption Orchards

Life history Food Distribution Management... 98

Comparison of Two Yellow Sticky Traps for Capture of Western Cherry Fruit Fly (Rhagoletis indifferens) in Tart Cherry in Northern Utah 2015

Inshore wrasse pot fishery What are the issues?

Instructions: CLASSIFY ANIMALS AS BLOOD FEEDERS OR NON-BLOODFEEDERS 1. Take out the ADULT CARDS, and place them on the correct boxes 2.

Page 1 of 7. Development of a rat bait with slug-repellent properties 1. July 14, 2016

Escape tactics used by bluegills and fathead minnows to avoid predation by tiger muskellunge

West Coast Rock Lobster. Description of sector. History of the fishery: Catch history

Chapter 5: Methods and Philosophy of Statistical Process Control

2008 Excellence in Mathematics Contest Team Project B. School Name: Group Members:

THE CIRCULATION IN THE NORTERN PART OF THE DENMARK STRAIT AND ITS VARIABILITY ABSTRACT

400-Meter Hurdle Theory By Ralph Lindeman

PEDALING ANALYSIS Pedaling Analysis monitor the power output variations coordinated action the smoothness of the pedaling

The Changes Observed in Atlantic Bonito (Sarda sarda) Populations During the Autumn-Winter Migration in the Suthern Black Sea Coast

"Think Like A Pro" Test

PMT. Smaller species of annelid do not have gills. Explain why these small worms do not need gills to obtain sufficient oxygen

Transcription:

IBIS 130: 39-44 Individual differences in bait-fishing by the Green-backed Heron Ardeola striata associated with territory quality HIROYOSHI HIGUCHI Laboratory of Forest Zoology, Faculty of Agriculture, The University of Tokyo, Tokyo 113,Japan, and Museum of Zoology, The University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, Michigan 481 09, USA Accepted 9 December 1986 Individual differences were found in the feeding sites, frequency of bait fishing, and fishing success in three individual Green-backed Herons in southern Japan. The differences in the use of bait fishing and in fishing success seemed to be related primarily to differences in feeding sites, which were associated with territory quality. Bait fishing was used most frequently by the individual which fished often in open water with fewer suitable rocks, where the heron has to overcome the handicap of being easily seen by fish. Bait fishing was least common in the individual which fished most often from high branches. The leaves and twigs dropped from high branches rarely reached the spot the heron intended, and the fishing success was low. Individuals probably also differed in their skill at bait fishing. Bait fishing, i.e., fishing with the use of bait by the Green-backed Heron Ardeola striata is reported from southern USA (Love11 1958, Sisson 1974, Norris 1975, Keenan 1981, Preston et al. 1986), southern Japan (Higuchi 1986) and western Africa (Boswall 1983a,b, Walsh et al. 1985). It would be interesting to know why the behaviour appears to be restricted to certain areas within the vast distribution of the species. However, we have to collect much more data on the behaviour to clarify the reason. The data should include the development of the behaviour, factors affecting the frequency of the behaviour, and the features of the places where the behaviour is used. This paper presents the results of observations of individual differences in bait fishing by Green-backed Herons in Japan. Here I report the feeding sites, frequency of bait fishing, kind of bait and effectiveness of bait fishing in three individuals. It is suggested that individual differences in the frequency of bait fishing and the fishing success are mainly related to their territory quality and feeding sites. It is also reported that in most cases the time needed to catch a fish after throwing the bait is much shorter than reported in Higuchi (1986). Methods The study was conducted at a pond in Suizenji Park, the same area as in Higuchi (1986), in Kumamoto (32"45'N, 130"45'E), Kyushu. The pond occupies about lso/, of the 7.6 ha park. Maples and pines are scattered in the park, and ornamental rocks of various sizes are placed in the pond. Both native and introduced species of fish live in the pond; these include pale chub Zacco platypus, dace Tribolodon hakonensis, carp Cyprinus carpio, gudgeon Pseudogobio esocinus and golden fish Carassius auratus. Observations were made with binoculars ( x 8) and a scope ( x 20) on 14-1 6 May, 8-9 June, 29-3 1 July and 27-29 August 1985. No birds were banded, but three adult individuals could be distinguished by their leg colour: one bright orange (individual A), one pale yellow (B), and the other pinkish (C). The leg colour did not change during the study period. These three individuals were usually the only herons that used the pond as their main feeding place. The maximum number of adult herons feeding at the same time in the pond was four.

40 H. HIGUCHI IBIS 130 Three observation points, 20-30 m apart, were visited in turn every hour or two. The feeding points of each individual were recorded on a 1 : 1,500 scale map. One individual was observed for 30 min in a single observation sequence, although there were a few incomplete sequences of less than 30 min when the heron left the pond. Time spent in fishing with and without bait was measured in minutes in every observation sequence. When the heron was fishing with bait the kind of bait, fishing success, and time to catch a fish after the bait was thrown was noted. The catching time was measured using a stopwatch to 0.01 s. In analysing data, bait materials were grouped into two classes, i.e., live bait and lure bait. Live bait included flies and other insects, which can be eaten by fish. They were considered to be alive when caught by herons, though it was not certain whether the items were alive or not when used as bait. Lure bait included parts of plants, such QS twigs and leaves, and feathers, which are not eaten by fish. Some unidentified objects were also included here. They appeared to be pieces of twigs and leaves. Although the type of bait used and fishing success may be expected to differ by season (Higuchi 1986) and feeding site (e.g., by the height of the site) for each individual, the data were insufficient to analyse these questions. The frequency of use of different types of feeding sites was not investigated in the field. It was estimated later from timed observations recorded on field maps. Territory and feeding sites Results Each of the three individuals occupied an exclusive feeding territory in the pond (Fig. l), from which other individuals were expelled. When the owner was absent part of the territory could be invaded. The shape and size of each territory remained almost the same throughout the observation period. The three territories were somewhat different from each other in their environmental features. There were many rocks and bushes of various sizes in the territory of individual C. These rocks were 0.5-1.5 m wide and 0.1-1.5 m high from 0 15 30 rn Koktn,denju.noma - Figure 1. Territories of three individual Green-backed Herons in the pond (shaded area) of Suizenji Park, Kyushu. Each territory was drawn by connecting the outermost observation points at which each individual was not threatened by its neighbours.

1988 BAIT-FISHING BY HERONS 41 the surface of the water. The bushes were 1.5-3-5 m high, with some branches as low as 0.4-1.0 m from the surface of the water. There were fewer rocks and bushes in the territories of A and B. In particular, small rocks between 0.1 and 0.3 m high and bushes with lower branches less than 1.0 m above the water were scarce, except those on the edge of pathways. Moreover, in more than 80% of the territory of B the water was too deep for a heron to stand, while A could stand comfortably in more than 70% of the territory. The three individuals tended to forage at different types of sites. Although detailed measurements were not made (see Methods), individual A spent more than 70% of its foraging time fishing in the water, and the rest of the time fishing from branches 1.5-3.0 m above the water. B spent about 80% of its foraging time fishing from branches 1.5-2.0 m above the water, and the rest of the time fishing from the bank of the pond and from 0.5 to 1 *Om high rocks. C spent about 60% of its foraging time fishing from branches 04-0.6 m above the water, and the rest of the time fishing from 0.1 to 0.3-m-high rocks. Frequency of use of bait The frequency of bait fishing was significantly different between the three individuals (Table 1; Mann-Whitney U=25, z= -7.187 for A w. B; U= 105, z = - 2.669 for A 'u. C; U= 21, z = - 4.578 for B w. C; P < 0.01 in all cases, twotailed). Individual A spent 83.5% of its foraging time on an average using bait fishing, while B used bait fishing only 14.3% of the time. C was intermediate, using bait fishing 63.0% of the time on average. The frequency of use of live bait and lure bait was not significantly different between the three individuals [Table 2; Fisher's exact probability test, two-tailed (FEPT hereafter)]. Every individual used about 80% lure bait and 20% live bait. For all individuals, more than 80% of the lure bait were twigs, leaves and berries, and more than 90% of the live bait were flies and other insects. Additional bait materials that were not reported in Higuchi (1986) include a small dragonfly, pieces of mushroom (individual A) and a tiny stone (C). The size of bait could not be investigated in detail, but six leaves and two feathers used by A and three leaves used by B were apparently too large to lure fish. Those objects were more than 4 cm in length. All the objects used by C were less than 2 cm in length. Table 1. The frequency of use of bait fishing by three individual Green-backed Herons Percentage of time spent in fishing with bait without bait Observation sequences* Individual n Range Mean + s.d. Range Mean s.d. A 39 33.3-100 83.5 + 20.6 0-66.7 13.6t 19.5 B 34 0-73'3 14.3+19.8 26.7-100 83.2521'7 C 11 33.3-100 63.0+ 23.2 0-66.7 30.6 t 22.7 * Most of the observation sequences were 30 min long. For a few sequences of less than 30 min, the ratio of fishing with and without bait was weighted by the length of the observation sequences.

42 H. HIGUCHI IBIS 130 Efflciency of bait fishing Individual C was most successful at catching fish with bait (Table 2). The success of C when using lure bait (70*070) was significantly higher than that of A (6.4%) and B (1 1.1 yo) (FEPT, P= 0.001 for A w. C and P= 0.030 for B v. C), though the success rate when using live bait was not significantly different. There were no significant differences in fishing success with different bait materials between A and B. The differences in fishing success between live and lure baits were only significant in A (19.4% w. 6.4%) (FEPT, P=0*047). The high efficiency of bait fishing by C can be seen in Table 3, which shows the number of bait and fish taken per hour as well as the number of bait per fish. C caught about 2.6 fish with 3.8 pieces of bait per hour, while A used 10.8 pieces of bait to catch approximately one fish per hour, and B used 4.5 pieces of bait to catch 0.8 fish per hour. The average number of pieces of bait used to catch one fish was significantly smallerinc(1.4) thanina(ll.o)andb(s.s)(fept, P=O.001 foraw. C, P=O.O34 for B v. C), but the efficiency of bait use of A and B was not significantly different. Table 4 shows the efficiency of fishing with and without bait in the three individuals. The number of fish caught per unit time is the best index of the efficiency of fishing without bait, because it is difficult to determine the duration of individual feeding trials in which bait is not used. In A, the number of fish caught per 10 min while bait fishing was significantly larger than the number caught while fishing without bait (Mann-Whitney U= 273, z = - 2.454, P < 0.05, two-tailed). In B and C, the success rate was not significantly different between fishing with and without bait. The success rate while bait fishing was also significantly greater in C than in Aor B (Mann-Whitney U= 109, z = - 2.744 for Av. C; U= 37, z = - 2.697 for B v. C; P < 0.01 in both cases, two-tailed). Table 2. Frequency of use and effectiveness of two classes of bait by three individual Green-backed Herons Live bait Lure bait Total bait used n n Individual n ("zo of total) Yo success (O0 of total) "; success A 176 36 19.4 140 6.4 (20.5) (79.5) B 11 2 50.0 9 11.1 (18.2) (81.8) C 13 3 66.7 10 70.0 (23.1) (76.9) Table 3. Eficiency of bait fishing by three individual Green-backed Herons Hours of Bait used Fish caught Bait/hour Bait/fish Fishihour Individual observation n n n n n A 16.28 176 16 in.81 11.0 0.98 B 2.44 11 2 4.51 5.5 0.82 C 3.47 13 9 3.75 1.4 2.59

I 988 BAIT-FISHING BY HERONS 43 Table 4. Comparison of fishing eficiency with and without bait by three individual Green-backed Herons No. of fish caught per 10 rnin in fishing with bait without bait Observation Observation sequences' sequences' Individual n Range Mean k s.d. n Range Mean f s.d. A 39 0-0'95 0.18+0.29 20 0-0.67 0.03 +0.15 B 15 0-1'43 0.14+0.39 34 0-1.25 0.19k0.28 C 11 0-2.00 0.56 k 0.58 9 0-1.67 0.26 k 0.57 ' The length of observation sequences differs from case to case, depending on the ratio of fishing with and without bait (see Table 1). Table 5. Time in seconds needed to catch afish after throwing the bait by three individual Green-backed Herons Live bait Lure bait Individual n Range Meanks.d. n Range Mean+s.d. A 5 0.66-3'65 1.334+ 1.297 5 0'47-11'94 3.612k4.916 B - - - 2 1.11-8.95 5.030 C 4 0.62-1.36 0.850+0.343 5 0.56-1'18 0.772 k0.241 Time needed to catch fish The time needed to catch a fish after throwing the bait was difficult to measure accurately, and could be measured only in some of the successful bait fishing attempts (Table 5). The mean time when using live bait was 1.33 s and 0.85 s in individuals A and C, respectively, from which comparable data were obtained. The mean time when using lure bait was 3.61 s and 0.77 s in A and C, respectively. The time variation as well as the mean time was shorter in C. However, these differences in time were not significant either between A and C or between live and lure baits in each individual (Mann-Whitney U test). The time to catch fish after throwing the bait was generally shorter in this study than in Higuchi (1986). Particularly in 14 of the 21 cases reported here, the time was less than 1 s. Such short times were not recorded in the earlier study. This difference may be due to increased accuracy of time measurement with the use of a stopwatch in the present study or behavioural differences between the individuals studied in the two observation periods. It appears, however, that some of the earlier measurements were not accurate. Discussion The differences in fishing success between the three individuals were apparently related to their territory quality and feeding sites. There were several rocks and bushes suitable for fishing in the territory of C, which showed a high fishing success. This heron could select low branches and rocks, where it could conceal its body from

44 H. HIGUCHI IBIS 130 fish, drop the bait accurately on the water, and strike fish easily. Since these herons take advantage of the high sensitivity of fish to the dropping of bait (Higuchi 1986), it is important for them to drop the bait accurately at the spot where they intend to catch (i.e., very close to the fish). Individuals A and B had fewer good feeding sites. A often fished in the water, where its body was easily seen by fish and its fishing success was low in spite of frequent use of bait. B fished mostly from branches because of the depth of the water in most of its territory. This heron had to use high branches available there, and its fishing success was low, even with the use of bait, because the leaves and twigs rarely reached the spot where the heron intended to dive. The relationship between fishing success, territory quality, and feeding sites seems to account for the individual differences in the frequency of bait fishing. Individual A, which often fished in open water, had to use bait frequently to overcome the handicap of being easily seen by fish. This bird was sometimes observed to throw baits successively to the same spot. Its fishing success was greatly improved by the use of bait (see Table 4). This individual spent 84% of its foraging time in bait fishing. For B, which often fished from high branches, bait fishing was not effective, and fishing without bait was as successful as bait fishing. This heron spent only 14% of its foraging time in bait fishing. Individual C, which had good feeding sites, could easily improve its fishing success with the use of bait, but also appeared to be able to achieve a relatively high success without bait. This heron spent 63y0 of its foraging time in bait fishing. However, it is improbable that all the individual differences in fishing success and in the frequency of bait fishing can be explained by the territory quality and feeding sites. For example, some leaves and feathers used by A and B were too large to lure fish, while all the objects used by C were of appropriate size as lures. In addition, the mean and variation of time taken to catch fish with bait were smaller in C than in A and B. These facts suggest that C was more skilful in bait fishing. Skilfulness may be related to age and length of experience. Unfortunately, there are no data on the age of the three individuals. It is not known whether the difference in their leg colour reflects a difference in their age. Hancock & Elliott (1978) described the leg colour of Green-backed Herons as follows: legs yellowish in immature, more dusky or greenish yellow in adults and bright yellow, orange or reddish orange in the nuptial phase. Females tend to be a little less brightly coloured. The three individuals reported here were all in full adult plumage. I should like to dedicate this paper to the late Mr Teruo Sakanashi, who gave me information on Greenbacked Herons bait fishing and assisted in fieldwork. Dr Robert B. Payne and Mr Richard 0. Prum provided useful comments on the draft, and Mr Steven M. Goodman helped data analysis. This work was supported in part by a grant-in-aid for the Special Research Project on the Biological Aspects of Optimal Strategy and Social Structure from the Japan Ministry of Education, Science and Culture (No. 60107002). References BOSWALL, J. 1983a. Tool-using and related behaviour in birds: more notes. Avic. Mag. 89: 94108. BOSWALL, J. 1983b. Tool-using and related behaviour in birds: yet more notes. Avic. Mag. 89: 17C181. HANCOCK, J. & ELLIOTT, H. 1978. The Herons of the World. New York: Harper & Row. HIGUCHI, H. 1986. Bait-fishing by the Green-backed Heron Ardeola striato in Japan. Ibis 128: 285-290. KEENAN, W. J. 1981. Green Heron fishing with mayflies. Chat 45: 41. LOVELL, H. B. 1958. Baiting of fish by a Green Heron. Wilson Bull. 70: 28C281. NORRIS, D. 1975. Green Heron (Butorides oirescens) uses feather lure for fishing. Am. Birds 29: 652454. PRESTON, C. R., MOSELEY, H. & MOSELEY, C. 1986. Green-backed Heron baits fish with insects. Wilson Bull. 98: 613414. SISSON, R. F. 1974. Aha! It really works! Nat. Geogr. 144: 142-147. WALSH, J. F., GRUNEWALD, J. & GRUNEWALD, B. 1985. Green-backed Herons (Butorides striatus) possibly using a lure and using apparent bait. J. Om. 126: 439442.