FIELD VERIFICATION OF PREDATOR ATTRACTION TO MINNOW ALARM SUBSTANCE

Similar documents
Conditioned Alarm Behavior in Fathead Minnows (Pimephales promelas) and Test Their Ability

WARREN K. YUNKER, DAN E. WEIN, and BRIAN D. WISENDEN* Department of Biological Sciences University of Alberta Edmonton, Alberta, Canada T6G 2E9

ANTIPREDATOR RESPONSE TO INJURY-RELEASED CHEMICAL ALARM CUES BY CONVICT CICHLID YOUNG BEFORE AND AFTER INDEPENDENCE FROM PARENTAL PROTECTION

Brian D. Wisenden Æ Julie Pogatshnik Æ Danfee Gibson Æ Lucia Bonacci Æ Adam Schumacher Æ Allison Willett

Environmental Biology of Fishes (2005) 72: Ó Springer 2005

EFFECTS OF PREDATION ON THE BEHAVIOR OF GAMMARUS MINUS

Digital ESF. SUNY College of Environmental Science and Forestry. Devon Shaw. Joe Rubenstein. Shane Currey. Andrew Koch

Fish Survey of Arctic Lake (ID # ), Scott County, Minnesota in 2012

The nose knows: minnows determine predator proximity and density through detection of predator odours

Escape tactics used by bluegills and fathead minnows to avoid predation by tiger muskellunge

LEARNED RECOGNITION OF PREDATION RISK BY Enallagma DAMSELFLY LARVAE (ODONATA, ZYGOPTERA) ON THE BASIS OF CHEMICAL CUES

The relationship between the spatial distribution of common carp and their environmental DNA in a small lake

Threat-sensitive responses to disturbance cues in juvenile convict cichlids and rainbow trout

Relative Size Selectivity of Trap Nets for Eight Species of Fish'

BENSON PARK POND FISH SPECIES

2014 Island Lake Survey June 13 th, 2014 Andrew Plauck District Fisheries Biologist Report Prepared 4 March 2015

Cedar Lake Comprehensive Survey Report Steve Hogler and Steve Surendonk WDNR-Mishicot

Largemouth Bass. Max 30 inches Average 4-12 inches EATS

Indiana Administrative Code Page IAC Aquaculture permit Authority: IC Affected: IC Sec. 17. (a) A person must not

The Round Goby Botulism Connection. Renea A. Ruffing Graduate Research Assistant Penn State University

Figure 1. A) Sam Malan watches wasp choice test. B) Y-tube apparatus example.

The impact of fish predation on invasive rusty crayfish (Orconectes rusticus) populations in two northern temperate lakes

Quemahoning Reservoir

The Response of Naive Channel Catfish to Chemical Cues Associated with Predation

Comparative Survival of Pellet-Reared Muskellunge Stocked As Fingerlings In Bluegill Ponds With and Without Largemouth Bass

Arizona Game and Fish Department Region VI Fisheries Program

JadEco, LLC PO BOX 445 Shannon, IL 61078

The Prevalence of Different Species of Fish in Four Different Habitats of Douglas Lake

MISSISSIPPI LEGISLATURE REGULAR SESSION 2007

Predation Cycle-5 parts

Predation Cycle-5 parts

An Assessment of the Fish Community in Lake Acworth

Lake information report

Key Words: Attraction, Color Cue, and Wavelength. Introduction

Fashion a Michigan Fish

Inventory # Perch Lake

Student Worksheet: River Health and Indicator Species

Proposed Reclassification of Deer Creek, North Platte River Basin, Wyoming

Fish Community and Aquatic Ecosystem Responses to the Cessation of Eurasian Watermilfoil Chemical Treatment on Lake Ellwood, Wisconsin

Oregon Hatchery Research Center January 2014 David L. G. Noakes, Professor & Director

NORTHWEST SCIENCE AND INFORMATION

Improving post-stocking survival of hatchery reared threatened fish species

Fighting for Life in French Creek

LAKE DIANE Hillsdale County (T8-9S, R3W, Sections 34, 3, 4) Surveyed May Jeffrey J. Braunscheidel

Fish Survey of Goose Lake (ID # ), Ramsey County, Minnesota in 2012

My Key to Manitoba Fish

Investigating reproduction and abundance of bighead carp (Hypophthalmichthys nobilis) and silver carp (H. molitrix) in the Greenup pool, Ohio River

Spring Lake 2017 REEL FACTS Keith Meals Fisheries Biologist

Dangerously bold Featured scientist: Melissa Kjelvik from Michigan State University

NEVADA DEPARTMENT OF WILDLIFE STATEWIDE FISHERIES MANAGEMENT

DOW# West Twin Lake

EFFECTS OF PREDATION ON FOUNTAIN DARTERS STUDY

Objective. Materials and Methods

Lake information report

Lake LeAnn 2018 Fish Survey

Proposed Reclassification of Cherry Creek, North Platte River Basin, Wyoming. October 25, 2010

ASSESSMENT OF WHITE PERCH IN LAKE WINNIPESAUKEE, TUFTONBORO (2016) Anadromous and Inland Fisheries Operational Management Investigations

Bode Lake - South Population Survey

Elk Lake, Antrim and Grand Traverse counties T. 28, 29 N., R. 8, 9 W., Sec. many. Lake surveys. began at 40 feet

Epidermal club cells do not protect fathead minnows against trematode cercariae: a test of the anti-parasite hypothesis

Paulding Soil & Water Conservation District Fish Sale Guide

REEL FACTS. Regulations. Limblines Fishing with limblines and set hooks is prohibited at Lake Washington.

7/29/2011. Sport fish. Rough fish. Fish Population Assessment

BA1 BA2 BA3 BA4 BA5 BA6 CP1 CP2 CP3 CP4 CP5 CP6

Fish Survey of Parkers Lake (ID # ), City of Plymouth, Hennepin County, Minnesota in 2013

Tampier Lake Population Survey

Willett Pond Fish Survey

Introduction: JadEco, LLC PO BOX 445 Shannon, IL 61078

2011 Haha Lake Northern Pike Control

Responses of wild fishes to alarm chemicals in pristine and metal-contaminated lakes

Rat Cove and Brookwood Point littoral fish survey, 2002

Pond Management. Dan Lekie Agriculture & Natural Resource Agent K-State Research & Extension Johnson County

Bonita Creek Fish Monitoring November 4 6, 2015

LAKE SURVEY REPORT DRAFT VERSION PRELIMINARY DATA (AS OF 08/24/2018) Fisheries Management. DOW Number: Survey ID Date: 08/21/2018

Dangerously bold Featured scientist: Melissa Kjelvik from Michigan State University

Regulations Scavenger Hunt

Factors Affecting the Reproduction of Bluegill Bream and Largemouth Black Bass in Ponds

STUDY PERFORMANCE REPORT

Wampum Lake Population Survey

Fisheries Survey of Saratoga Lake

This project was completed: 6/30/2006

Conewago Creek Initiative. Fish Survey Report for the Conewago Creek

and found that there exist a significant overlap between the billfish resources and the exploitation activities targeting tunas and mahi mahi.

Diet Study of Walleye in Black Lake Using Isotope Analysis of d15n and d13c

Big Bend Lake Population Survey

Transforming Chadwick Lake

FISH COMMUNITIES AND FISHERIES OF THE THOUSAND ISLANDS AND MIDDLE CORRIDOR

Michigan Dept. of Natural Resources Status of the Fishery Resource Report Page 1

Colorado River Fishery Project

Name That Fish : Identify living things using an existing classification key, and explain the rationale used.

NEVADA DEPARTMENT OF WILDLIFE STATEWIDE FISHERIES MANAGEMENT

Busse Reservoir South Lateral Pool Population Survey

Annotated Checklist of the Fishes of the Rio Grande Drainage, Dona Ana, El Paso, and Hudspeth Counties

Trophy Bass Management

Current projects for Fisheries Research Unit of Minnesota Department of Natural Resources

Manual of Fisheries Survey Methods II: with periodic updates. Chapter 22: Guidelines for Sampling Warmwater Rivers with Rotenone

Mille Lacs Lake Creel Survey Report for. Open Water Season of and. Winter Season of

Pennsylvania Fish & Boat Commission Biologist Report. Wilmore Dam. Cambria County. May 2011 Trap Net, Electrofishing and Hoop Net Survey

~ A Behavioral Response Study in 2007 &2008 (BRS 07/08) was conducted in the Bahamas to

Kemper County Lake 2019 Reel Facts Trevor Knight Fisheries Biologist (662)

Transcription:

Journal of Chemical Ecology, Vol. 28, No. 2, February 2002 ( C 2002) FIELD VERIFICATION OF PREDATOR ATTRACTION TO MINNOW ALARM SUBSTANCE BRIAN D. WISENDEN* and TRAVIS A. THIEL Biology Department Minnesota State University, Moorhead Moorhead, Minnesota 56563 (Received April 16, 2001; accepted September 11, 2001) Abstract Fishes such as minnows in the superorder Ostariophysi possess specialized alarm substance cells (ASC) that contain an alarm cue. Alarm substance can only be released by damage to the epidermis; thus, the release of alarm substance is a reliable indicator of predation risk. When nearby minnows detect the cue, they adopt a range of antipredator behaviors that reduce their probability of predation. Predator predator interactions afford prey an opportunity to escape and, thus, a fitness benefit that maintains alarm substance calls over evolutionary time. Here, we present data from a simple field experiment verifying that nearby predators are attracted to minnow alarm substance because it signals an opportunity to pirate a meal. Fishing lures were baited with sponge blocks scented with either (1) water (control for sponge odor and appearance), (2) skin extract from non-ostariophysan convict cichlids (superorder Acanthopterygii, Archocentrus Cichlasoma nigrofasciatus) to control for general injury-released cues from fish, or (3) skin extract from fathead minnows (superorder Ostariophysi, Pimephales promelas). Predator strike frequency on each sponge type was 1,1, and 7 for water, cichlid, and minnow cues, respectively. These data provide the first field test using fish predators of the predator-attraction hypothesis for the evolution of Ostariophysan alarm substance cells. Key Words Schreckstoff, alarm substance, alarm pheromone, minnow, predator attraction, evolution, chemical signal, predator prey, Ostariophysi. INTRODUCTION Predation, and risk of predation, influences the evolution of behavior and morphology of virtually all animals because predation is the final arbiter of natural To whom correspondence should be addressed. E-mail: wisenden@mnstate.edu 433 0098-0331/02/0200-0433/0 C 2002 Plenum Publishing Corporation

434 WISENDEN AND THIEL selection (Lima and Dill, 1990). Chemical cues mediate predator prey interactions in aquatic habitats for organisms ranging from ciliates to mammals (Wisenden, 2001). Chemical cues released by predators, or from conspecifics that have been injured or frightened, are used by prey to detect and evade predators (Smith, 1992, 1999; Kats and Dill, 1998; Chivers and Smith, 1998; Wisenden, 2000). Predators cause physical damage and injury to their prey in the process of capture, handling, and ingestion. Consequently, chemical cues from injured prey are often released during a predator attack, and these cues serve as public information about predation risk. Most aquatic taxa, from ciliates to amphibians, exhibit antipredator behavior in response to injury-released chemical cues from conspecifics (Chivers and Smith, 1998). Fishes in the superorder Ostariophysi, i.e., minnows, catfish, characins, and suckers, represent about 64% of all freshwater fish species (Nelson, 1994). One character that unites this group is the presence of specialized epidermal cells that contain an alarm substance; hence, the cells are referred to as alarm substance cells (ASCs). These cells lack an external duct. The cell contents are released only when the skin is damaged. Predator attacks on Ostariophysans release alarm substance into the surrounding water and warn nearby prey of predation risk. Alarm substance is potent. Minnow skin elicits overt antipredator behavior when diluted to 1 cm 2 skin per 58,000 liters (Lawrence and Smith, 1989), or the equivalent volume of a sphere with a radius of 3.7 m. The behavioral response to this cue significantly reduces the probability of predation risk (Mathis and Smith, 1993). ASCs have caught the attention of evolutionary ecologists. It is not readily apparent how the alarm signaling function of ASCs provides a selfish benefit to the signal sender, and thereby provides a mechanism for these cells to be maintained by natural selection (Smith, 1992; Williams, 1992). The only evolutionary mechanism for ASCs for which there is experimental support is the predator-attraction hypothesis (Smith, 1992). In a laboratory experiment, northern pike Esox lucius were more attracted to ASC minnow skin extract than skin extract without ASCs (Mathis et al., 1995). Similarly, in a field experiment, dytiscid predaceous diving beetles were more attracted to traps scented with minnow skin with ASCs than traps scented with minnow skin that lacked ASCs (Mathis et al., 1995). Chivers et al. (1996) showed in staged predation events in laboratory aquaria that fathead minnows in the grasp of a small pike have a significant chance of escape if a second, slightly larger, pike is allowed to interfere. Fathead minnows receive a selfish fitness benefit by investing in ASCs because ASCs attract secondary predators, not because they warn conspecifics of predation risk. The studies by Mathis et al. (1995) and Chivers et al. (1996) make important contributions to the understanding of the evolution of chemical alarm signaling in this ecologically important group of fishes. No other experiments

MINNOW ALARM SUBSTANCE 435 have been conducted to verify these findings, and importantly, they have not been verified with fish predators in the field. In this study, we report the results of a simple field experiment that provides further support for the secondary predatorattraction hypothesis. METHODS AND MATERIALS We conducted our experiment on Lake Ida, approximately 25 km north of Alexandria, Minnesota, USA (46 00 N, 95 25 W). Potential minnow predators in Lake Ida are: northern pike, Esox lucius; walleye, Stizostedion vitreum vitreum; largemouth bass, Micropterus salmoides; yellow perch, Perca flavescens; black crappie, Poxomis nigromaculatus; several types of bullhead catfish, Ameiurus melas, A. nebulosus, A. natalis; and perhaps bluegill sunfish, Lepomis machrochirus. We fished for 4 hr on January 27 and 4 hr on February 11, 2001. Ice cover at the time of data collection was about 80 cm thick. We drilled six holes about 25 cm in diameter with a gas-powered auger and spaced the holes about 8 m apart. We used a depth sonar to standardize water depth below all holes at about 3 m. We fitted each hole with an ice-fishing tip-up. A tip-up has a small platform and an upright stand connected to an arm that extends over the hole. The arm was equipped with a reel of anti-freezing line followed by a 1.1 m length of transparent monofilament line leader. A lure was attached to the end of the leader. At the other end of the arm there was a piece of springy metal attached to a fluorescent orange flag. The flag was flexed against the reel of line in such a way that when a fish tugs on the line the flag is released, signaling a strike. In addition, a broad metal paddle was attached to the arm designed to catch any wind movement, causing the arm to bob up and down and jig the line. Jerky vertical motion on the line attracts fish to the lure. All holes were about 300 m from shore and equally exposed to wind. By using wind-powered tip-ups, we standardized the visual presentation of test stimuli among treatments. Each tip-up was fitted with a fluorescent green, 1/32 oz (0.9 g) teardrop style jig and a small block of cellulose sponge (1 1 0.7 cm). The sponge block was soaked with one of three test solutions: (1) dechlorinated tap water to control for the odor and appearance of the sponge and background attractiveness of the jig lure, (2) skin extract of a tropical fish (convict cichlid, Archocentrus Cichlasoma nigrofasciatus) to control for general chemical cues released from injured fish, or (3) skin extract of fathead minnows, Pimephales promelas. Convict cichlids are not in the Ostariophysi (superorder Acanthopterygii) and, therefore, do not possess Ostariophysan ASCs. Skin extracts for each species were prepared using the same protocol. We used five cichlids (mean ± SE = 6.8 ± 0.8 cm, N = 5)

436 WISENDEN AND THIEL and 11 minnows (mean ± SE = 6.5 ± 0.17 cm, N = 11) to make the skin extract solutions. We killed each fish by a blow to the head, then carefully filleted the skin from each flank. Skin fillets were measured, then placed in chilled dechlorinated tap water. The total area of skin collected was 68.44 cm 2 and 66.97 cm 2 for cichlids and minnows, respectively. To simulate a predator attack we macerated the skin fillets with a conventional hand-held blender. The solution was filtered through polyester wool to remove scales, connective tissue, and suspended tissue particles. Dechlorinated water was added to bring the total volume of each skin extract to 32 ml. We made 64 sponge blocks of each odor type. Each sponge block was infused with 0.5 ml of dechlorinated water, cichlid skin extract, or minnow skin extract. Therefore, cichlid and minnow cues were of equivalent strength, based on 1.0 cm 2 of skin extract per sponge for each species cue. We froze the sponges at 20 C until needed. Conspecifics exhibit an alarm reaction to skin extract that has been frozen for both fish species used in this experiment (Lawrence and Smith, 1989; Wisenden and Sargent, 1997). Of the six holes fished each day, two were assigned water sponges, two were assigned cichlid sponges, and two were assigned minnow sponges. We replaced the sponges with fresh ones every 30 min. We also rotated locations so that each sponge type was used at every location during each test day. We recorded the number of times that the tip-up arms showed a clear tip, i.e., the tip of the arm pulled into the hole, and the number of times the indicator flags were released. We analyzed the data by using a chi-square goodness of fit test. Conventional wisdom maintains that a chi-square test cannot be used if more than 25% of the cells contain frequencies less than 5. However, this oft-repeated rule of thumb is overly conservative and unnecessary (Haldane, 1943; Snedecor and Cochran, 1980, p. 77). RESULTS Over the course of 48 hole-hr of fishing effort, holes with the minnow-sponges received seven predator strikes, of which six released the flag, while holes with the cichlid-sponges and water-sponges received one strike (and flag release) each (χ 2 = 8.0, P < 0.02). Tip-ups baited with the scent of injured minnows were more attractive to predators than general chemical cues released by an injured non-ostariophysan fish (χ 2 = 4.5, P < 0.05). Predator attraction to the cichlid cue was not different from water (P = 1). The time between replacement of the sponge and strike on the lure varied and did not seem related to treatment type. Lures scented with minnow skin were struck 6 24 min (mean ± SE = 14 ± 6 min, N = 4) after sponge replacement. The single strike on the cichlid-scented lure occurred 4 min after sponge replacement. Strike time was not recorded for the water-scented strike.

MINNOW ALARM SUBSTANCE 437 DISCUSSION The first conclusion from this experiment is that chemosensory cues are important in eliciting strike behavior. Because minnows are small fishes and fall prey to a wide variety of predators, it is reasonable to infer that fish large enough to trip the tip-up flag were minnow predators. The second conclusion is that not all scents are equally effective. The strike frequency on the cichlid cue was the same as for the water cue, indicating that general substances released from injured fish (amino acids, proteins, lipids, etc.) do not automatically elicit a strike response from predators. Minnow scent was clearly preferred by predators, consistent with predictions from chemical alarm signaling theory (Smith, 1992; Mathis et al., 1995; Chivers et al., 1996). Therefore, this study provides the first field verification from fish predators of the predator-attraction hypothesis for the evolution of ASCs. The predator species that struck the scented lures could not be observed through the ice; however, Lake Ida is best known for its pike fishery during the ice-fishing season. None of the fish striking the scented lures were landed because the sponge block was on the jig hook. Nevertheless, fish large enough to release the tip-up flag would be large enough to eat a fathead minnow or interrupt a predation event by attempts of piracy or cannibalism. The precise chemistry of the alarm cue contained in minnow skin cells is not completely understood. A nucleotide, hypoxanthine-3(n)-oxide, has long been thought to be the active ingredient in the alarm cue/predator attractant (unpublished PhD thesis by Argentini in 1976, after Smith 1999). Laboratory (Pfeiffer et al., 1985; Mathis et al., 1995) and field (Brown et al., 2000) tests confirm the biological activity of hypoxanthine-3(n)-oxide. An alternative explanation for the differences in strike frequency is that predators may have preferred minnow cue because minnow cue is familier to them. Cichlids are tropical fish and allopatric to the fish species in Lake Ida. However, there is no evidence to suggest that the chemical cues released by cichlids should differ qualitatively from those typical of general non-ostariophysan fishes. Cichlids are in the same order, Perciformes, and ecologically similar to centrarchids (sunfish, bass), which are abundant in Lake Ida. The most likely explanation of the differences in strike frequency in the current experiment is that skin from minnows produces a more potent cue, with perhaps better dispersing properties than an equivalent area of cichlid skin. The presence of ASCs in minnow skin is the most likely cause of this. Acknowledgments Dennis Thiel assisted in data collection. Tip-ups were donated by Scheel s All Sports of Fargo. Funding was provided by MSU Moorhead s Dille Fund for Excellence. Statistical advice was provided by Dr. Jerry Stockrahm, Math Department, MSU Moorhead.

438 WISENDEN AND THIEL REFERENCES BROWN, G. E., ADRIAN, J. C., JR., SMYTH, E., LEET, H., and BRENNAN, S. 2000. Ostariophysan alarm pheromones: Laboratory and field tests of the functional significance of nitrogen oxides. J. Chem. Ecol. 26:139 154. CHIVERS,D.P.andSMITH, R. J. F. 1998. Chemical alarm signalling in aquatic predator/prey systems: A review and prospectus. Ecoscience 5:338 352. CHIVERS, D. P., BROWN, G. E., and SMITH, R. J. F. 1996. The evolution of chemical alarm signals: Attracting predators benefits alarm signal senders. Am. Nat. 148:649 659. HALDANE, J. B. S. 1943. The use of χ 2 as a test of homogeneity in a (n 2)-fold table when expectations are small. Biometrika 33:234 238. KATS, L. B. and DILL, L. M. 1998. The scent of death: Chemosensory assessment of predation risk by prey animals. Ecoscience 5:361 394. LAWRENCE, B. J. and SMITH, R. J. F. 1989. Behavioral response of solitary fathead minnows, Pimephales promelas, to alarm substance. J. Chem. Ecol. 15:209 219. LIMA, S. L. and DILL, L. M. 1990. Behavioural decisions made under the risk of predation: A review and prospectus. Can. J. Zool. 68:610 640. MATHIS, A. and SMITH, R. J. F. 1993. Chemical alarm signals increase the survival time of fathead minnows (Pimephales promelas) during encounters with northern pike (Esox lucius). Behav. Ecol. 4:260 265. MATHIS, A., CHIVERS, D. P., and SMITH, R. J. F. 1995. Chemical alarm signals: Predator deterrents or attractants? Am. Nat. 146:994 1005. NELSON, J. S. 1994. Fishes of the World, 4th ed. Wiley-Interscience, New York. PFEIFFER, W., RIEGELBAUER, G., MEIER, G., and SCHEIBLER, B. 1985. Effect of hypoxanthine-3(n)- oxide and hypoxanthine-1(n)-oxide on central nervous excitation of the black tetra Gymnocorymbus ternetzi (Characidae, Ostariophysi, Pisces) indicated by dorsal light response. J. Chem. Ecol. 11:507 523. SMITH, R. J. F. 1992. Alarm signals in fishes. Rev. Fish. Fish Biol. 2:33 63. SMITH, R. J. F. 1999. Avoiding and deterring predators, pp. 163 190, in J.-G. J. Godin (ed.). Behavioural Ecology of Teleost Fishes. Oxford University Press, New York. SNEDECOR, G. W. and COCHRAN, W. G. 1980. Statistical Methods, 7th ed. Iowa State University Press, Ames, Iowa. WILLIAMS, G. C. 1992. Natural Selection: Domains, Levels, and Challenges. Oxford University Press, New York. WISENDEN, B. D. 2000. Scents of danger: The evolution of olfactory ornamentation in chemicallymediated predator-prey interactions, pp. 365 386, in Y. Espmark, T. Amundsen, and G. Rosenqvist (eds.). Animal Signals: Signalling and Signal Design in Animal Communication. Tapir Academic Press, Trondheim, Norway. WISENDEN, B. D. 2001. Chemically-mediated strategies to counter predation, in S. P. Collin and N. J. Marshall (eds.). Sensory Processing in the Aquatic Environment. Springer-Verlag, New York. In press. WISENDEN, B. D. and SARGENT, R. C. 1997. Antipredator behavior and suppressed aggression by convict cichlids in response to injury-released chemical cues of conspecifics but not to those of an allopatric heterospecific. Ethology 103:283 291.