Theory of the Hunter-Covey Interface

Similar documents
Impact on Base Population Density and Hunter Performance of Stocking with Pen-Raised Bobwhite

Factors Influencing Northern Bobwhite Hunting Success on Two South Georgia Plantations

Population Parameters and Their Estimation. Uses of Survey Results. Population Terms. Why Estimate Population Parameters? Population Estimation Terms

IMPROVING POPULATION MANAGEMENT AND HARVEST QUOTAS OF MOOSE IN RUSSIA

Do Resident and Non-Resident Northern Bobwhite Hunters Self-Regulate Harvest Based on Population Size?

PREDATOR CONTROL AND DEER MANAGEMENT: AN EAST TEXAS PERSPECTIVE

Introduction. Introduction 4/4/2011. Northern bobwhite (Colinus virginianus) population decline. Breeding Bird Survey

OREGON DEPARTMENT OF FISH AND WILDLIFE SUMMARY OF COUGAR POPULATION MODEL AND EFFECTS OF LETHAL CONTROL

Deer Census - How to Use It to Calculate Harvest

NORTHERN BOBWHITE HUNTING: BEHAVIOR OF HUNTERS AND DOGS GRANT ELLIOT MECOZZI. Bachelor of Science. University of Alaska Fairbanks.

Demographics of Quail Hunters in Oklahoma

MANAGING GEESE WITH RECREATIONAL HUNTING

MAXIMUM ECONOMIC YIELD AND RESOURCE ALLOCATION IN THE SPINY LOBSTER INDUSTRY* Joel S. Williams and Fred J. Prochaska

Report to the Joint Standing Committee on Inland Fisheries and Wildlife

Chapter 20. Sampling the Recreational Creel

What s A Quail Worth? A Longitudinal Assessment of Quail Hunter Demographics, Attitudes, and Spending Habits In Texas

Monitoring Population Trends of White-tailed Deer in Minnesota Marrett Grund, Farmland Wildlife Populations and Research Group

Status and Distribution of the Bobcat (Lynx rufus) in Illinois

A pheasant researcher notebook:

Oh Deer! Objectives. Background. Method. Materials

Traffic safety developments in Poland

DMU 005 Antrim County Deer Management Unit

Status Report on the Yellowstone Bison Population, August 2016 Chris Geremia 1, Rick Wallen, and P.J. White August 17, 2016

DMU 065 Ogemaw County Deer Management Unit

Development of Biomimicry Wind Louver Surface Design

Minnesota Deer Population Goals

The Traditional Southern Bobwhite Quail Plantations

THE WOLF WATCHERS. Endangered gray wolves return to the American West

QUAIL PARASITES FOUND AT RECORD-HIGH LEVELS IN WEST TEXAS ACCORDING TO NEW RESEARCH PROJECT

F I N D I N G K A T A H D I N :

Summary of discussion

DMU 056 Midland County Deer Management Unit

The Response of Bobwhite Conveys to Disturbance During Field Trials

Boyle s law Verifying the relation between air pressure and volume measuring air pressure in a closed container.

Traffic circles. February 9, 2009

DMU 082 Wayne County Deer Management Unit

Susitna-Watana Hydroelectric Project Document ARLIS Uniform Cover Page

DMU 332 Huron, Sanilac and Tuscola Counties Deer Management Unit

MODELLING OF FUME EXTRACTORS C. R.

Recommendations for Pennsylvania's Deer Management Program and The 2010 Deer Hunting Season

I'd like to thank the Board for the opportunity to present here today.

DMU 045 Leelanau County Deer Management Unit

2015 Deer Population Goal Setting

CHECKS AND BALANCES. OVERVIEW Students become managers of a herd of animals in a paper-pencil, discussionbased

Deer Management Unit 255

Findings of the Alaska Board of Game BOG

Growth: Humans & Surf Clams

5/DMU 069 Otsego County Deer Management Unit

Influence of Food Plots on Nothern Bobwhite Movements, Habitat Use, and Home Range

Northwest Parkland-Prairie Deer Goal Setting Block G7 Landowner and Hunter Survey Results

A Hare-Lynx Simulation Model

A Study of the Relationship between Horse Slaughter and Reported Cases of Abuse and Neglect

DMU 046 Lenawee County Deer Management Unit

PARAMETRIZATION OF WAVE TRANSFORMATION ABOVE SUBMERGED BAR BASED ON PHYSICAL AND NUMERICAL TESTS

1. What function relating the variables best describes this situation? 3. How high was the balloon 5 minutes before it was sighted?

Minnesota Deer Population Goals. East Central Uplands Goal Block

EE 364B: Wind Farm Layout Optimization via Sequential Convex Programming

Central Hills Prairie Deer Goal Setting Block G9 Landowner and Hunter Survey Results

Management of Montezuma Quail in Texas: Barriers to Establishing a Hunting Season

AN ASSESSMENT OF NEW JERSEY DEER HUNTER OPINION ON EXPANDING ANTLER POINT RESTRICTION (APR) REGULATIONS IN DEER MANAGEMENT ZONES 28, 30, 31, 34 AND 47

EEB 122b PRACTICE SECOND MIDTERM

Deer Management Unit 152

Author s Name Name of the Paper Session. Positioning Committee. Marine Technology Society. DYNAMIC POSITIONING CONFERENCE September 18-19, 2001

Contingent Valuation Methods

Fall Wild Turkey Population Survey, 2010

2000 AP ENVIRONMENTAL SCIENCE FREE-RESPONSE QUESTIONS

The North American Waterfowl Management Plan: Incorporating the New Goal

Designing a Traffic Circle By David Bosworth For MATH 714

Survey Techniques For White-tailed Deer. Mickey Hellickson, PhD Orion Wildlife Management

Deer Management Unit 252

Discussion on the Selection of the Recommended Fish Passage Design Discharge

Gamblers Favor Skewness, Not Risk: Further Evidence from United States Lottery Games

Biologist s Answer: What are your goals? Deer Management. Define goals, objectives. Manager s Question: Should I cull or shoot spikes?

Name Class Date. What are some properties of gases? How do changes of pressure, temperature, or volume affect a gas?

DMU 008 Barry County Deer Management Unit

Fifty years ago, a single cod was large enough to feed a family of four or five. Today it is barely enough for one

I. What is a Fishery? II. What is Fisheries Management? III. What is Fisheries Science? I. Brief history of the evolution of fisheries science.

American Society of Mammalogists

DMU 361 Fremont Deer Management Unit Newaygo, Oceana, N. Muskegon Counties

2018 Season Waterfowl Hunter Survey Summary. Presented by Josh Richardson, Sr. Biologist Oklahoma Department of Wildlife Conservation

DMU 043 Lake County Deer Management Unit

DMU 024 Emmet County Deer Management Unit

Report of Raccoon Dog management in Finland for 2016

2008 Excellence in Mathematics Contest Team Project B. School Name: Group Members:

2) Jensen, R. Comparison of ground-reaction forces while kicking a stationary and non-stationary soccer ball

DMU 073 Saginaw County Deer Management Unit

LEAPS BOUNDS. Growing up hunting as a kid in New Hampshire, I didn t. by Dan Bergeron

USING BIOLOGICALLY IMPORTANT PHYSICAL CHARACTERISTICS OF ESTUARIES TO CLASSIFY AUSTRALIAN AND NEW ZEALAND ESTUARIES

Effects of Incentives: Evidence from Major League Baseball. Guy Stevens April 27, 2013

Deer Harvest Characteristics During Compound and Traditional Archery Hunts

A Model for Tuna-Fishery Policy Analysis: Combining System Dynamics and Game Theory Approach

USATF 2008 SUPER CLINIC CRITICAL GENERAL PERFORMANCE DESCRIPTORS FOR THE SHORT HURDLES. Ralph Mann, Ph.D.

Gas Pressure. Pressure is the force exerted per unit area by gas molecules as they strike the surfaces around them.

DMU 072 Roscommon County Deer Management Unit

The University of Georgia

DMU 053 Mason County Deer Management Unit

Matching respondents over time and assessing non-response bias. Respondents sometimes left age or sex blank (n=52 from 2001 or 2004 and n=39 from

13.1!"#$#%"&'%()$*+%,+-.$+/*$#

Full summaries of all proposed rule changes, including DMU boundary descriptions, are included in the additional background material.

RECRUITMENT HUNTERS A case-study approach to learning more about hunting among Hispanics and improving recruitment and retention of other hunters

Transcription:

National Quail Symposium Proceedings Volume 4 Article 17 2000 Theory of the Hunter-Covey nterface Andrew A. Radomski Texas A&M University Fred S. Guthery Texas A&M University Follow this and additional works at: http://trace.tennessee.edu/nqsp Recommended Citation Radomski, Andrew A. and Guthery, Fred S. (2000) "Theory of the Hunter-Covey nterface," National Quail Symposium Proceedings: Vol. 4, Article 17. Available at: http://trace.tennessee.edu/nqsp/vol4/iss1/17 This Analyzing the Effects of Predation and Hunting is brought to you for free and open access by Trace: Tennessee Research and Creative Exchange. t has been accepted for inclusion in National Quail Symposium Proceedings by an authorized editor of Trace: Tennessee Research and Creative Exchange. For more information, please contact trace@utk.edu.

Radomski and Guthery: Theory of the Hunter-Covey nterface THEORY OF THE HUNTER-COVEY NTERFACE Andrew A. Radomski Caesar Kleberg Wildlife Research nstitute, Texas A&M University-Kingsville, MSC 218, Kingsville, TX 78363-8202 l Fred S. Guthery 1 Caesar Kleberg Wildlife Research nstitute, Texas A&M University-Kingsville, MSC 218, Kingsville, TX 78363-8202 ABSTRACT We established mathematical models and explored the role of a learned response (avoidance behavior) to understand and manage the hunter-covey interface. Furthermore, we examined the dynamic nature of the probability of flush, given encounter, in a population that learned to avoid hunters as time passed. Learning rate was defined as the proportion of a covey that leaves the naive population and enters the experienced population per unit of hunter-covey contact. The conditional probability of flushing and shooting at a covey, given a covey encounter, declined through the season. This is because the probability of flushing was lower for experienced than for naive coveys and the population of experienced coveys grew with exposure. Thus, quality of hunting declined at a faster rate than quail population; i.e., birds became more wary as the hunting season progresses. The birds' ability to avoid hunters provided an explanation of the sudden reappearance of bobwhites contributing to reproduction in areas hunters were unsuccessful the previous hunting season. Management can use our models to manipulate the interface and obtain a desired population following the hunting season. Citation: Radomski, A.A., and FS. Guthery. 200 Theory of the hunter-covey interface. Pages 78-81 in L.A. Brennan, W.E. Palmer, L.W. Burger, Jr., and T.L. Pruden (eds.). Quail V: Proceedings of the Fourth National Quail Symposium. Tall Timbers Research Station, Tallahassee, FL. NTRODUCTON Wildlife biologists and hunters have long recognized avoidance behavior by northern bobwhites ( Colinus virginianus), i.e., trap shyness and flighty behaviors. Thorndike (1911), an early animal behaviorist, formulated this concept as the Law of Effect. t states "[O]f several responses made to the same situation, those which are accompanied or closely followed by satisfaction to the animal will, other things being equal, be more firmly connected with the situation, so that, when it recurs, they will be more likely to recur." n other words, a response followed by favorable consequences becomes more probable than a response followed by unfavorable consequences. Additionally, individuals that live in groups (e.g., bobwhite coveys) may have the opportunity to learn to recognize unfamiliar dangers by observing the responses of experienced individuals in the group. This behavioral concept is termed cultural transmission (Mainardi 1980, Curio 1988, Mineka and Cook 1988). Cultural transmission of information has been reported for several avian species, in which predator-naive individuals learn to recognize predators by observing the responses of experienced birds (Klopfer 1957, Curio et al. 1978, Vieth et al. 1980). Several assumptions in animal behavior are: ( 1) all behavior is caused or determined in some way; i.e., all behavior obeys certain laws; (2) explanations of behavior based on internal causes and mental states are generally useless; and (3) the environment molds be- 1 Present address: Department of Forestry, 008C Agriculture Hall, Oklahoma State University, Stillwater, OK 74078. Published by Trace: Tennessee Research and Creative Exchange, 2000 78 havior. Animal behaviorists explain the cause of behavior by studying only those behaviors that can be observed and measured, without reference to unobservable mental processes. Covey dynamics for northern bobwhites in southern Texas have been reported (Lehmann 1984). The nature of the behavior process, termed the hunter-covey interface, was explored because hunter-harvest data are commonly used in bobwhite density estimates, management, and establishing future hunting regulations. We used mathematical models to predict possible outcomes of the learning process on the huntercovey interface. We started with a simple static model of daily harvest and generalized the model to account for avoidance behavior by bobwhites and site selection by hunters. The theoretical background for understanding the hunter-covey interface provides information that can be incorporated into harvest management plans. METHODS We first established and developed a static model of daily harvest. Under the static model 1 hunting party hunts 1 area on 1 day. The number of birds harvested (K) on any day is the product of coveys encountered times the number of birds shot per covey encountered. This statement may be expressed as (1) K = mp(nls) m = mean number of birds shot per covey flushed, p = probability of encountering a covey, 1

National Quail Symposium Proceedings, Vol. 4 [2000], Art. 17 HUNTER-COVEY 79 N = total bobwhite population at the beginning of the day, and s = average covey size on the day. The probability of encountering a covey (p) is of considerable practical and theoretical interest. Conceptually, we may view a given hunt as an area covered superimposed on an area hunted. Therefore, a hunt will effectively cover an area of some size within a larger area available for hunting. Assuming (1) hunting pressure is nonredundant, i.e., new space is hunted at each instant, and (2) coveys are randomly distributed in space on the hunted area, then the probability of encountering any covey is the area covered on the hunt divided by the area available for hunting, (2) p = a/a a = the area (ha) effectively hunted on a day, and A = the area (ha) available for hunting. The area effectively hunted increases with the speed of the hunters, the time spent hunting, and the effective width of the hunting zone. The relationship can be described as: (3) a= vhw v = the velocity at which hunters travel (linear units/hr), h = hours spent hunting, and w = the effective width of the hunting zone (linear units). The width (w) is homologous to effective strip width in line transect sampling of wildlife density. The time for hunting (h) is limited, and more or less fixed to morning and evening. However, the velocity may be increased by hunting from vehicles, horses, and/or by use of dogs. Rosene (1969:347) estimated a hunter on foot with dogs covers about 120 ha per day, as hunting parties using horses or vehicles cover 400-600 ha per day. f one assumes hunters using vehicles flush 3-5 times as many coveys as hunters on foot, then it is possible for low quail densities to be associated with high time-rates of flushing (coveys per hr) when hunters increase velocity and width. The above arguments lead to a more general model of daily kill as: (4) K = m(vhw/a)(nls). This simple model holds under random distribution of coveys and nonredundant hunting pressures, which limits the model's application. More realistic models could incorporate avoidance behavior (learning) by coveys (Sisson 1996), which may be counteracted by hunters with selection of better habitat patches for hunting, baiting, or both. Also, encountering a covey relates to how a covey responds (freeze, fly, run) when a hunting party approaches. The probability of flushing a covey also must address if it is within shooting range. The word flush, in the context of our paper, means hunters flush a covey within shooting range. However, there will be a fraction of the coveys flushed because of avoidance behavior (all coveys encountered will not flush). Then the probability a covey flushes, given encounter, is defined as Pt Under avoidance behavior, we revise the previous equation to: (5) K = m(vhw/a)p/nls). The above equation is subject to the assumption hunters do not preferentially select portions of areas for hunting and they do not bait. However, whether hunters bait, preferentially select hunting sites, or both, is not of concern in a more general conceptual model of daily harvest. Preferential site selection is conceptually similar to baiting in model development. We can define an area of size B which is preferentially selected and/or baited within the general area of size A. We specify all hunting occurs within area B and hunters show no preferential use within area B. Then the probability of encountering a covey becomes conditional on Ph, the probability a covey occurs in area B. Note that p" relates specifically to the bobwhite population and not to area. The model for the daily kill now becomes: (6) K = m(vhw!b)p 1 p1,,(nls). This equation provides a deterministic estimate and therefore is best considered an average value under the conditions specified. Also, we realize that certain variables in the general model are dynamic. For example, scenting conditions for dogs vary with temperature and humidity (Gutzwiller 1990), which imposes variation in the effective width of the hunting zone (w). Populations decline through the hunting season as does the mean number of birds in coveys. And behaviorally, bobwhites may become more wary as time passes and exposure to hunting continues. Next, we explored learning behavior leading to hunter-avoidance. n particular, we examined the dynamic nature of the probability of flush, given encounter, in a population that learns to avoid hunting parties over time (t). Naive coveys C" were defined as not being exposed to hunting pressure. We assumed all coveys were naive at the start of the hunting season, and the probability of flush, given encounter, was lower for experienced coveys than for naive coveys; i.e., experienced coveys showed hunter-avoidance behavior. ndividuals from naive coveys were lost through harvest, natural mortality, and emigration into the population of experienced coveys. The population of experienced coveys acquired gains from ingress of naive individuals and losses from harvest and natural mortality. Population dynamics of the naive and experienced coveys can be modeled with similar natural mortality rates and harvest-loss rates per hunter-covey contact. The dynamic variable of interest is the mean probability of flush, given encounter, at some time t, defined as pf.,. Since hunters are less likely to flush an experienced covey than a naive covey, this mean is a weighted average of pfe and Pte at time t, http://trace.tennessee.edu/nqsp/vol4/iss1/17 2

Radomski and Guthery: Theory of the Hunter-Covey nterface 80 RADOMSK AND GUTHERY j: :::; ~ 0 "' 1. 0:: Cl. z ili :; o.i 20 40 ;/ LOW-LOW 60 80 OF HUNTNG SEASON 100 Fig. 1. Modeled trends in the mean probability of flush given an encounter for a bobwhite population consisting of naive and experienced coveys. Descriptors refer to hunting-intensity-learning rate; i.e., low-low indicates low hunting intensity and low learning rate. Experienced coveys have a lower conditional probability than naive coveys. The figure provides qualitative information on the dynamics of the mean probability. (7) Pt., = (pfncn., + PteCe_,)/(Cn., + Ce_,) Pt., = average probability of flush given encounter for a population containing naive and experienced coveys on day t, Ptn = probability of flush given encounter for naive coveys, Pte = probability of flush given encounter for experienced coveys, Cn., = total population of naive coveys on day t, and Ce., = total population of experienced coveys on day t. The dynamics of naive and experienced covey populations may be defined in differential form as (8) en. t+l Cn., - jpcn_,h - kpp 1 ncn_,h - lcn., Cn., (l - jph - kpp~ - l) and (9) Ce.,+ 1 = Ce., + jpcn., H - kpp 1 ece., H - Zee., j = the learning rate or rate at which coveys become experienced for each hunter-covey contact, k = the loss rate to harvest for each hunter-covey contact, l = the daily loss rate to nonhunting mortality, and H = the number of hunting parties. The coefficients may be considered as some fraction of a covey per unit of hunter-covey contact (j, k) or per covey (l). RESULTS The mean probability of flush, given encounter, initially declined as experienced coveys increased as a proportion of the total population (Figure 1). t was possible for the mean to stabilize at some value under high hunting pressure and a high learning rate. This stabilization occurred when the population was saturated with experienced coveys; i.e., coveys that encountered hunters during the hunting season. The dynamic model revealed several qualitative outcomes of the hunter-covey interface under avoidance behavior (Figure 2). The population of naive coveys may only decline, as that of experienced coveys may grow throughout the hunting season. Naive coveys declined more rapidly as hunting pressure and learning rate increased; conversely, experienced coveys increased more rapidly to peak populations during the season as hunting pressure and learning increased. Under high hunting pressures and learning rates, it was possible for a population to consist entirely of experienced coveys for a large portion of the season (see HGH-HGH graph, Figure 2). Temporal trends in the daily harvest would be similar to those for the mean probability of flush given encounter (Figure 1). Under these models, the total population declined continuously because of natural mortality. Likewise, the daily kill would decline continuously (holding hunting pressure constant) because the kill represents some fraction of the total population. Trends in daily kill will appear flat with low harvest rates, learning rates, and natural mortality. Trends will appear more spiked as these variables increase. DSCUSSON Empirically observed values form range between 1.5 and 2.0 birds downed per covey flushed. Bennitt (1951) reported an average of 1.86 (SD = 076) for hunters in Missouri. Harvest data from a southern Texas corporate hunting lease, which included 2 hunters per covey flush, indicate m = 1.68 (SD = 572) (unpublished data). The static model provides intuitive methodology for increasing or decreasing daily harvest or time available for hunting. This model predicts daily harvest declines as take per flush, velocity of travel, time spent hunting, width of the hunting zone, and the probability of flush, given encounter, decline (holding the population of coveys constant). Hunting time required to obtain a specified harvest on a given day varies inversely with the product of the 4 variables under management control, m, v, w, and Pr; i.e., the time required to meet the specific harvest goals increases in a hyperbolic fashion as the product of these variables decreases linearly. The model resulting in qualitative analysis of the dynamic interface between hunter and covey warns against general statements concerning the effects of avoidance behavior. According to the model, effects are contingent on the learning rate and hunting pressure (Figure 1). Trends in the probability of flush, given encounter, under LOW-LOW and HGH-LOW regimes illustrate the potential effects of hunting pressure under identical learning rates. Moreover, the prob- f Published by Trace: Tennessee Research and Creative Exchange, 2000 3

National Quail Symposium Proceedings, Vol. 4 [2000], Art. 17 HUNTER-COVEY 81 (/) 50 \\\ LOW-LOW w >- 0 TOTAL 0 50 (/) 40 > 30 > 300... \ \\ 200 -,NE~ 10 EXPERENCED 100 30 60 90 50 50 HGH-LOW (/) (/) 40 > > 0 0 30 ' 20 \., 20 100 /~ 10 30 60 90 LOW-HGH ~ Fig. 2. Modeled trends in populations of naive, experienced, and total coveys under different hunting intensities and quail learning rates during a 90-day season. Descriptors refer to hunting intensity-learning rate. The figure provides qualitative information on covey dynamics because values of variables in the model are unknown. ability of flush, given encounter, varies in a nonlinear manner over time. Under these models, the probability of flush, given encounter, was constant for experienced and naive coveys. Only the mean probability may vary with time, because of changes in the proportions of naive and experienced coveys. Therefore, there may exist periods within seasons the mean probability of flush, given encounter, is unaffected by hunting intensity and learning rate (all coveys are experienced). This occurred at about day 45 in the hypothetical HGH-HGH regime (Figure 1). The mean probability changed imperceptibly after day 45. MANAGEMENT MPLCATONS f the assumption is that harvest makes no difference in population dynamics of quail, then there is no applied value in our models. However, if this is not the case, then the daily kill models illustrate several variables amenable to management action; velocity, time, width, baiting, and kill/flush. For example, the latter variable may be influenced by restricting hunters to smaller gauge shotguns. Also, the models may be used to maximize recreation. That is, solving to maximize h could be attained by manipulating the remaining variables in the daily harvest model. Finally, knowledge of the dynamics of hunter-avoidance could be applied in managing harvest. One could start with naive hunters, say youth, to propagate wariness and then allow the more experienced hunters access. n theory, this would maximize recreation with some constraints on the total harvest. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS We thank T. Shupe for providing unpublished hunter-harvest data from a lease in South Texas. We acknowledge D. Hewitt, K. Nolte, F. Chavez-Ramirez, and N. Silvy for reviewing this manuscript. This manuscript is Caesar Kleberg Wildlife Research nstitute No. 99-113. LTERATURE CTED Bennitt, R. 1951. Some aspects of Missouri quail and quail hunting, 1938-48. Missouri Conservation Commission Technical Bulletin 2. Curio, E. 1988. Cultural transmission of enemy recognition in birds. Pages 75-98 in T.R. Zentall and B.G. Galef, Jr. (eds.). Social Learning: Psychological and Biological Perspectives. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, NJ. Curio, E.,U. Ernst, and W. Vieth.1978. The adaptive significance of avian mobbing.. Cultural transmission of enemy recognition in blackbirds: effectiveness and some constraints. Zeitschrift fur Tierpsychologie 48: 184-202. Gutzwiller, K. J. 199 Minimizing dog-induced bias in game bird research. Wildlife Society Bulletin 18:351-356. Klopfer, P.H. 1957. An experiment on emphatic learning in ducks. American Naturalist 91:61-63. Lehmann, V. W. 1984. Bobwhites in the Rio Grande Plains of Texas. Texas A&M University Press, College Station, TX. Mainardi, D. 198 Tradition and the social transmission of behaviour in animals. Pages 227-255 in G.W. Barlow and J. Silverberg (eds.). Sociobiology: Nature/Nurture. Westview Press, Boulder, CO. Mineka, S. and M. Cook. 1988. Social learning: psychological and biological perspectives. Pages 51-74 in T.R. Zentall and B.G. Galef, Jr. (eds.). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, NJ. Rosene, W. 1969. The bobwhite quail. Rutgers University Press, New Brunswick, NJ. Sisson, D. C. 1996. Efficiency of bird dogs in locating bobwhite coveys. Pages 49-55 in W.E. Cohen (ed.). Texas Quail Short Course. Texas Agricultural Extension Service, College Station, TX. Thorndike, E. L. 911. Operant conditioning. Pages 253-275 in J.W. Kalat (ed.). ntroduction to psychology, 2nd ed. Wadsworth Publishing Company, Belmont, CA. Vieth, W., E. Curio, and U. Ernst. 198 The adaptive significance of avian mobbing.. Cultural transmission of enemy recognition in blackbirds: cross-species tutoring and properties of learning. Animal Behavior 28:1217-1229. http://trace.tennessee.edu/nqsp/vol4/iss1/17 4