Bias and variability in distance estimation on the water: implications for the management of whale watching

Similar documents
A REVIEW OF EXISTING GUIDELINES, CODES OF CONDUCT AND REGULATION FOR WHALE WATCHING

National Report on Large Whale Entanglements

Concept for a Whale Protection Zone for the Endangered Southern Resident Killer Whale

2016 West Coast Entanglement Summary

Lifeforce Foundation Orca Conservation Programs

Movements of satellite-tagged false killer whales around the main Hawaiian Islands: implications for management

Puget Sound s whales face intertwined obstacles By The Seattle Times, adapted by Newsela staff Jul. 15, :00 AM

March 15, Dear Ms. Barre:

Acoustic and Visual Survey of Cetaceans at Palmyra Atoll

Puget Sound's whales face intertwined obstacles

AGENCY: National Marine Fisheries Service (NMFS), National Oceanic and Atmospheric

Biological & Behavioral Response Studies of. Marine Mammals in Southern California, BRS.org. Photos taken under NMFS permit #

US Dept. of Commerce NOAA National Marine Fisheries Service Office of Law Enforcement

August Wildlife Report. Boat Harassment of Orcas.

Coastal and marine recreation in New England is ingrained in the region s economic and

Summer Flounder. Wednesday, April 26, Powered by

OVERARCHING PRINCIPLES AND BEST PRACTICE GUIDELINES FOR MARINE MAMMAL WATCHING IN THE WIDER CARIBBEAN REGION (WCR)

FINAL 2015 SOUNDWATCH PROGRAM ANNUAL CONTRACT REPORT

211

Become a Marine Biologist for the Day

Fin Whale. Appendix A: Marine Wildlife. Balaenoptera physalus. New Hampshire Wildlife Action Plan Appendix A Marine-38. Federal Listing.

Wandering Whale Watches:

SCIENTIFIC COMMITTEE SEVENTH REGULAR SESSION August 2011 Pohnpei, Federated States of Micronesia

CALIFORNIA DEPARTMENT OF FISH AND WILDLIFE UPDATE ON LANDINGS OF TUNA, SWORDFISH AND OTHER PELAGICS

Modify Federal Regulations for Swordfish Trip Limits the Deep-set Tuna Longline Fishery. Decision Support Document November 2010

NATIONAL: SUPPORT FOR CIRCUS ANIMAL BAN

Drownings in Hawaii, A review of death certificates by the Injury Prevention and Control Program, Hawaii DOH

BAJA BLUE WHALE RESEARCH 2011

Potential collision risks of large whales and fast ferries traveling between Korea and Japan

Reducing Risk of Whale Entanglements in Oregon Dungeness Crab Gear

Map Showing NAFO Management Units

Season Summary on Large Whale Entanglement threat and reports received around the Main Hawaiian Islands

Monitoring the length structure of commercial landings of albacore tuna during the fishing year

Fine-scale Focal DTAG Behavioral Study in the Gulf of Maine

COASTAL & OFFSHORE PACIFIC CORPORATION 242 Rosa Corte Walnut Creek, CA Via

Groundfish Endangered Species Workgroup Report

Gallup on Public Attitudes to Whales and Whaling

World Shipping Council. National Oceanographic and Atmospheric Administration

The Growth and Socioeconomic Value of the Whale Watch Industry Worldwide. Mick McIntyre, Asia Pacific Director

The Economics of Atlantic Highly Migratory Species For-Hire Fishing Trips July - November 2013 Clifford Hutt and George Silva

Marine Mammals. James M. Price. Division of Environmental Sciences. from NOAA photograph library

Orange County MPA Watch A n n u a l R e p o r t

British Columbia s. Johnstone Strait & Blackfish Sound. Encounter Orcas, humpback whales, dolphins, porpoises, seals and sea lions

The 2001 Economic Benefits of Hunting, Fishing and Wildlife Watching in MISSOURI. Prepared by:

IN THE FEDERAL COURT OF AUSTRALIA NEW SOUTH WALES DISTRICT REGISTRY No. NSD 995 / 2005 HUMANE SOCIETY INTERNATIONAL INC SUMMARY OF RELEVANT FACTS

Legislation. Lisa T. Ballance Marine Mammal Biology SIO 133 Spring 2013

2013 SOUNDWATCH PROGRAM ANNUAL CONTRACT REPORT

Commercial Dungeness Crab Newsletter

WHALES. & Whale Sharks

Briefing Paper #1. An Overview of Regional Demand and Mode Share

DISENTANGLEMENT RESPONSE CHECKLIST

Fine-Scale Survey of Right and Humpback Whale Prey Abundance and Distribution

August 3, Prepared by Rob Cheshire 1 & Joe O Hop 2. Center for Coastal Fisheries and Habitat Research Beaufort, NC

ASMFC Stock Assessment Overview: Red Drum

REPORT OF THE WORKSHOP ON THE LEGAL ASPECTS OF WHALE WATCHING PUNTA ARENAS, CHILE NOVEMBER 1997 INTERNATIONAL FUND FOR ANIMAL WELFARE

Annual Pink Shrimp Review

DISTRIBUTION STATEMENT A. Approved for public release; distribution is unlimited.

Overview of Florida s Cooperative East Coast Red Snapper Tagging Program, SEDAR41-DW10. Submitted: 1 August 2014

Review of A Detailed Investigation of Crash Risk Reduction Resulting from Red Light Cameras in Small Urban Areas by M. Burkey and K.

University of Miami Rosenstiel School of Marine and Atmospheric Science. Billfish Research Program

An Application of Signal Detection Theory for Understanding Driver Behavior at Highway-Rail Grade Crossings

Distribution of the Atlantic Bottlenose Dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) in the Chesapeake Bay Drainage in Virginia

Abstract. Ulrik Röijezon, PhD and Paul Arden, MCCI

Harbour Porpoise Release Program Newsletter

Reduction of Speed Limit at Approaches to Railway Level Crossings in WA. Main Roads WA. Presenter - Brian Kidd

Angling in Manitoba Survey of Recreational Angling

Tonga Case Study - Taki Hausia & Olive Andrews

Gulf of the Farallones National Marine Sanctuary Safe Harbor for Sea Turtles

no-take zone 1 of 5 Channel Islands National Marine Sanctuary, California

Briefing on the IWC s Conservation Committee

BALEEN WHALE REPORTS FROM THE EASTERN ARABIAN SEA BASED ON INTERVIEW SURVEYS AND STRANDING REPORTS - UPDATE FROM INDIA

INTRODUCTION. Proceedings of the 2005 Puget Sound Georgia Basin Research Conference. Figure 1. Study Area (Graphic: Courtesy Puget Sound Action Team).

~ A Behavioral Response Study in 2007 &2008 (BRS 07/08) was conducted in the Bahamas to

Patterns of Southern Resident Killer Whale (Orcinus orca) Movement in Relation to the Tides and Currents

Some Biological Parameters of Bigeye and Yellowfin Tunas Distributed in Surrounding Waters of Taiwan

Mitigating odontocete depredation & by-catch in pelagic longline fisheries: physical & psychological deterrence at the hook.

BLUE, FIN AND SEI WHALE RECOVERY PLAN

The 2006 Economic Benefits of Hunting, Fishing and Wildlife Watching in TEXAS. Prepared by:

Sharing the Seas Safe Boating for Sailors and Whales. Information provided by Sharing the Seas for the 2017 Vineyard Cup

LEVEL M TABE 11 & 12 READING OTT. Whale Watching. Read the passage. Then answer questions 1 through 7.

TAGGING STUDY OF BLACK SEA BASS IN NEW JERSEY OCEAN WATERS PRELIMINARY RESULTS BY DEBORAH VAREHA AND BILL FIGLEY

Analyzing Human- Environment Interactions using GIS. Cape Breton Highlands Education Centre/Academy

Sheepshead Fishery Overview South Atlantic State/Federal Management Board May 2014 Introduction Life History Landings

FORMERLY THE NATIONAL COALITION FOR MARINE CONSERVATION (NCMC) Billfish Conservation Act Implementing Regulations; NOAA-NMFS

WHAT CAN WE LEARN FROM COMPETITION ANALYSIS AT THE 1999 PAN PACIFIC SWIMMING CHAMPIONSHIPS?

NOAA Fisheries Service (NMFS) Update on North Atlantic Right Whale Recovery Actions

MANAGEMENT ESTABLISHING JURISDICTION LEGAL BASIS DEFINING LOGICAL APPROACHES

Public Hearing Document

Three different funding sources funded different facets of the research.

TECHNICAL NOTE THROUGH KERBSIDE LANE UTILISATION AT SIGNALISED INTERSECTIONS

Golfers in Colorado: The Role of Golf in Recreational and Tourism Lifestyles and Expenditures

ASMFC Stock Assessment Overview: Red Drum

Re: Agenda Item I.2. - Deep-Set Buoy Gear Authorization

Discards of red grouper (Epinephelus morio) for the headboat fishery in the US Gulf of Mexico SEDAR 42- DW- 17

ATLANTIC STURGEON. Consultations on listing under the Species at Risk Act

The 2006 Economic Benefits of Hunting, Fishing and Wildlife Watching in NORTH CAROLINA. Prepared by:

CCAMLR MPA Edited June 2017

Comparison of EU and US Fishery management Systems Ernesto Penas Principal Adviser DG Mare

Admiralty Inlet Pilot Tidal Project FERC No Appendix C. Derelict Gear Monitoring Plan

Re: Algae/Cyanobacteria Bloom in St. Lucie, Martin, Palm Beach and Lee Counties.

Transcription:

IWC Meeting Document SC/52/WW1 (2000) Bias and variability in distance estimation on the water: implications for the management of whale watching Robin W. Baird Biology Department, Dalhousie University, Halifax, Nova Scotia, B3H 4J1, Canada (e-mail: rwbaird@is.dal.ca) Stephanie M. Burkhart Pacific Whale Foundation, 101 North Kihei Road, Kihei, Hawaii 96753, USA ABSTRACT Distance regulations or guidelines are the most common method used by regulatory agencies to manage interactions between whale watching vessels and whales. In Hawaiian waters, a distance rule prohibits vessels from approaching humpback whales (Megaptera novaeangliae) closer than 100 yards (91 m). Vessel captains and government enforcement agents rely almost entirely on estimating distance to whales, yet research on distance estimation abilities on land suggest a large degree of variability associated with such estimation. In 1999, we used laser range finders (accurate +/- 1 yard) to measure the distance between whale watching boats and whales, and obtained estimates of distances from vessel captains, naturalists, and members of the general public (702 estimate/measurement pairs from 106 individuals). We found a high degree of variability associated with distance estimation to whales on the water, more so than had been documented in land-based studies or controlled ship-based distance estimation experiments with highly experienced observers. A general bias towards under-estimating distance (ie., estimating that the vessels are closer to whales than they actually are) suggests that the distance rule is likely conservative, in terms of encouraging vessel operators to stay at least 100 yards from whales. Distance estimation abilities seem to improve with experience: estimates of vessel captains were less variable than estimates of naturalists or passengers. However, given the overall high degree of variability in estimating distance to whales, combined with the ease of use and relatively low cost of commercially available laser range finders, we suggest that such instruments should be incorporated into both whale watching and management of whale watching activities. INTRODUCTION With the rapid growth of whale watching worldwide, there is increasing concern over the shortand long-term impacts of vessel activity on whales (e.g., Bauer and Herman 1986; Duffus and Dearden 1993; IFAW et al. 1996). Since little information is available on the impacts of whale watching activities on whales, management agencies are faced with the difficult task of promulgating and enforcing regulations which will continue to allow whale watching, but which minimize disturbance to the animals. Distance guidelines or restrictions are the primary management tool used by agencies around the world to mitigate impacts of vessel activity around marine mammals (Carlson 1996). Although in many areas of the world only voluntary guidelines exist, distance approach regulations have been implemented in parts of Australia, New Zealand, the West Indies, South Africa and the United States. It is important to see not only how well these regulations are being enforced, but also how effectively vessel captains and enforcement personnel, as well as the general public, are able to interpret and follow these rules. This includes

determining how well people are able to accurately estimate distance on the water, as well as examining the factors influencing people s perception of distance. Several studies have been undertaken examining the biases in estimating distance on land, including the effect of training in increasing accuracy in distance estimation (Gibson and Bergman 1954; Gibson et al. 1955). Such studies have been undertaken in a controlled manner, using subjects of the same sex and age (usually young adult males), and examining distance estimation over land surfaces with a clear textural gradient. On the water, similar controlled experiments have been undertaken using experienced observers from ship-board surveys for cetaceans, with estimates made as a ship approaches floating radar buoys (Oien and Schweder 1992; Schweder 1996; 1997). Results from such studies are interesting, but it remains difficult to assess how the controlled and somewhat unrealistic circumstances presented (compared with an actual encounter with a whale in the wild) can be applied to real-life situations. Our study was developed to investigate the accuracy of distance estimation to whales by those directly involved in whale watching activities, including vessel captains, naturalists, and passengers. In particular, we have focused on the bias and error in distance estimation to whales as they relate to the effectiveness of distance regulations in the management of whale watching. STUDY SITE Each year, humpback whales migrate between summer feeding areas in high latitudes and winter mating and calving areas in low latitudes (Clapham 2000). In the North Pacific, the main (SE) Hawaiian Islands are one of the primary wintering grounds for humpback whales (Baker et al. 1986). While wintering in Hawaii, concentrations of humpback whales can be consistently found off the leeward (south and west) sides of the main Hawaiian Islands. Whale watching activities in Hawaii focus on humpback whales, and last from mid-december through April. During the 1999 season (mid-december 1998 through mid-april 1999) there were a total of 52 commercial vessels running an average of 87 whale watching trips per day in Hawaii (Utech 1999). Utech (1999) estimated that almost 370,000 individuals went whale watching in Hawaii during the 1999 whale watching season, generating between $11 and $16 million dollars in direct revenue. Of the four islands where whale watching activities are based, the majority of vessels operate from the island of Maui (Utech 1999). The humpback whale is listed as an Endangered species under the U.S. Endangered Species Act of 1973, and within 320 km (200 nautical miles) of the Hawaiian Islands is protected by a number of federal laws and regulations. Regulations prohibit the operation of aircraft within 305 m (1000 feet) of humpback whales, and restrict vessels from approaching closer than 91 m (100 yards), as well as prohibiting any act which would cause a vessel or other object to approach closer than 91 m (Nitta 1997). METHODS From January-May, 1999, passengers, captains and naturalists onboard whale watching vessels operating out of 2 ports (Lahaina and Ma alaea) on the island of Maui were asked to volunteer as part of a research study looking at people s perception of distance on the water. In order to determine whether any of the passengers had substantial prior experience with estimating distances on the water, passengers were asked to provide basic background information, including occupation (and if retired, their previous occupation) and sports that they play, and

were asked questions related to prior distance estimation experience (e.g., boating activities). All volunteers were at least 20 years of age. Each volunteer was asked to estimate the distance (in yards) between him/herself and a whale, while an independent observer simultaneously measured the actual distance (in yards) with laser range finders. Bushnell Yardage Pro 800 laser range finders were used to obtain measurements to whales at distances of up to 600 m (with a stated accuracy of ± 1 m). Each volunteer and rangefinder operator stood side by side, so no correction for difference in distance was required. Measurements obtained from different range finders off the same target were identical. For each volunteer, the height of the boat platform from which the observer was viewing the whales was noted (3 different boats, one with 2 decks, were used, giving two general heights, from 1-1.8 m above sea level, and approximately 5.5 m above sea level). The behavior of the whale 30 seconds prior to the estimate/measurement was recorded, and behavior was later divided into two categories: surface-active groups (involving breaches, lunges, tail-lobbing, etc), or non-surface-active groups. We attempted to get 10 measurements per volunteer. Estimates/measurements were made at least 5 minutes apart except when whales surfaced on opposite sides of the boat, so that volunteers were not influenced by their prior estimate. Volunteers were not given feedback on how close their estimates were to the measured distance, so as to not bias their future estimates. When describing the results we refer primarily to two different attributes of the data: the magnitude and direction of bias, and the amount of variability. The magnitude of bias describes the accuracy of estimates compared to measurements, and is calculated as the percentage of the actual distance (see Table 1). The direction of bias refers to the tendency to overestimate or underestimate distance. The variability indicates the scatter in estimated values for the same true distance, and is calculated as the percentage error (see Table 1). For measures of central tendency, we present both mean and median values, since the parameters are not always normally distributed (as no estimates can be less than 0% of the true distance, yet estimates can be greater than 200% of the true distance). In general we use all samples from all individuals, rather than use mean values for each individual, as the variability between individual means was far lower than the variability between samples. To test the effect of this on our conclusions, we compared treatments based on individual means for some tests, and found no difference in significance. RESULTS One hundred and six people participated in the study (58 men; 48 women) and 702 estimates/measurements were recorded. While some of the passengers sampled had some prior boating experience, none had previously spent time attempting to estimate distance to whales, so for the purposes of examining experience level, we have simply compared three groups: whale watch captains, naturalists and passengers. Overall, observers showed a bias towards underestimating distance, with mean and median estimates of 89% and 77% of the true distance, respectively (SD 60%, see Figure 1). The distance between the observer and whales did not affect this tendency (regression of percentage error versus measured distance, r 2 =0.0058, slope= -0.00057). The magnitude of bias and level of

variability were both affected by gender (Figure 1), with women being less accurate (more bias), but also showing less variability in their distance estimates than men. Experienced observers (naturalists and captains combined) of both sexes exhibited the same direction and magnitude of bias as inexperienced observers, though estimates were less variable (Figure 2; Table 1). Vessel captains were slightly better at estimating distances than naturalists, and both groups showed far less variability in their distance estimates than people with less experience (Table 1; Kruskal-Wallis one-way ANOVA on all samples, p < 0.001, on individual means, p < 0.001). Estimates made at a greater height above water were generally more accurate and less variable (Table 1), however these differences were not significant (Mann-Whitney U-tests on all samples, accuracy, p = 0.29; variability, p = 0.45). To rule out the potential influence of gender and/or experience, we also compared estimates for a subset of the data (the group with the largest sample size, male passengers - 46 individuals and 284 samples), and found similar results (Mann-Whitney U-tests, accuracy, p = 0.85, variability, p = 0.55). The general behavior of the whales (surface-active or not) did not appear to influence the variability of the estimates (Table 1; Mann-Whitney U-test, p = 0.36), however it did affect the accuracy of the estimates (Table 1; Mann-Whitney U-test, p = 0.001). Taking into account only those individuals where at least five samples were obtained and averaging the percentage error values for each individual, the mean level of variability was 37% (SD 20%, n=73 individuals). Some observers were relatively good at estimating distance (mean ± SD: 10%±7%), while others had much higher levels of variability (mean ± SD: 117%±60%). Twenty-seven percent of these observers always underestimated, while 10% always overestimated. Sixteen percent of the observers were just as likely to underestimate as they were to overestimate. DISCUSSION There is clearly a high degree of variability associated with distance estimation on the water (Figure 1). The degree of error found with both inexperienced passengers and experienced naturalists and vessel captains on the water (Figure 2) appears to be greater than distance experiments with individuals on land (Gibson and Bergman 1954), and prior controlled experiments on the water (Oien and Schweder 1992), although the bias in each case (a tendency to underestimate) was the same. In terms of distance regulations, such a bias towards underestimating results in a tendency to err on the side of caution, that is, individuals are likely to be greater than 100 yards from a whale when they think they are at 100 yards distance. This suggests that distance regulations may be a conservative approach to managing boat/whale interactions (assuming that keeping people a set distance away from whales is the management goal). Experience on the water was expected to make one better at estimating distance (Gibson and Bergman 1954; Gibson et al. 1955) and, not surprisingly, captains had the least variability and bias in their estimates (Table 1). Gender was another factor that influenced both the magnitude of bias and degree of variability of distance estimates, though it is unclear why females would be less variable (and less accurate) than males (cf. Kimura 1999). When individuals were at a higher platform on a boat, they were less variable in their estimates, and

less likely to overestimate, though these differences were not significant. Behavior of the whales (surface-active versus non-surface active) seemed to influence the magnitude of bias, with observers being more accurate with surface-active groups. Whales which are surface-active tend to show more of their bodies above the water, and observers might thus be able to use the whale s size as a reference in estimating distance. In general, people are relatively poor at estimating distances to whales. Given that estimating distance is currently the main way vessel captains try to comply to distance regulations, this should be a matter of concern, both to management agencies and to vessel captains. The range finders we used were relatively inexpensive and easy to use, and we recommend that whale watching operators purchase such range finders for use on the water. Alternatively, given that distance estimation abilities improve with experience, on-the-water training and practice using range finders could be done with vessel captains throughout the whale watching season. Problems in whale watching management associated with distance estimation are not limited to vessel captains, however. Enforcement agents of the National Marine Fisheries Service, or deputized agents from other federal (e.g., Coast Guard) or state agencies (e.g., the Department of Land and Natural Resources in Hawaii) all rely on estimating distances between boats and whales when determining whether a violation of distance regulations has occurred, and in most cases such agents likely have far less experience at judging distances to whales than do captains of whale watching vessels. Given the tendency of all groups to underestimate distance (Table 1), it is likely that in many cases vessels may actually be compliant with regulations when enforcement agents think otherwise. The problems with whale watching management do not stop there. Members of the general public, particularly those on shore, often view near-shore interactions between boats and whales. We suspect that the error in estimating distance between two objects offshore is even greater than what we have reported here, thus land-based reports of vessels violating distance regulations should be viewed with particular caution. The majority of boat-based whale watching that occurs in the nearshore-waters of Maui, Hawaii, appears to occur from commercial whale watching vessels, but our results are also relevant to recreational whale watching. In some parts of the United States, private recreational boaters make up a substantial proportion of the boats found around whales (e.g., off San Juan Island in Washington state, at times over two-thirds of the boats with killer whales, Orcinus orca, are recreational, R. Otis and R.W. Osborne, personal communications). Given the relatively poor abilities of inexperienced individuals (a category which we suspect the majority of recreational boaters fall into) in estimating distances to whales, we believe it is doubly important that tools such as range finders be used in educational programs and management efforts. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS Pacific Whale Foundation Eco-Adventures allowed us to collect data from their boats. A. Roberts and the PWF 1999 Hawaii Humpback Whale Project interns assisted with data collection. R.W. Baird was supported during data collection by the Pacific Whale Foundation, and during writing by a grant from the Ocean Futures Society to Dalhousie University. The manuscript was improved from constructive comments by P. Cabe, J. Gordon, S.K. Hooker, R.W. Osborne and T. Schweder.

REFERENCES Baker, C. S., L. M. Herman, A. Perry, W. S. Lawton, J. M. Straley, A. A. Wolman, G. D. Kaufman, H. E. Winn, J. D. Hall, J. M. Reinke and J. Ostman. 1986. Migratory movement and population structure of humpback whales (Megaptera novaeangliae) in the central and eastern North Pacific. Marine Ecology Progress Series 41:103-114. Bauer, G. B., and L. M. Herman. 1986. Effects of vessel traffic on the behavior of humpback whales in Hawaii. Contract report to the National Marine Fisheries Service, Honolulu, Hawaii. Carlson, C. A. 1996. A review of whale watching guidelines and regulations around the world. Report for the International Fund for Animal Welfare, Crowborough, United Kingdom. Clapham, P. J. 2000. The humpback whale: seasonal feeding and breeding in a baleen whale. In J. Mann, R. Connor, P. Tyack and H. Whitehead, editors. Cetacean societies: field studies of dolphins and whales. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, Illinois, USA. Duffus, D. A., and P. Dearden. 1993. Recreational use, valuation, and management, of killer whales (Orcinus orca) on Canada s Pacific coast. Environmental Conservation 20:149-156. Gibson, E. J., and R. Bergman. 1954. The effect of training on absolute estimation of distance over the ground. Journal of Experimental Psychology 48:473-482., R. Bergman, and J. Purdy. 1955. The effect of prior training with a scale of distance on absolute and relative judgments of distance over ground. Journal of Experimental Psychology 50:97-105. IFAW, Tethys Research Institute and Europe Conservation. 1996. Report of the workshop on the scientific aspects of managing whale watching, Montecastello di Vibio, Italy. International Fund for Animal Welfare. Nitta, E. T. 1997. An overview of the responsibilities and activities of the National Marine Fisheries Service Section 7 consultations, approach regulations, and research. National Oceanic and Atmospheric Administration Technical Memorandum NMFS-OPR-11:74-80. Oien, N., and T. Schweder. 1992. Estimates of bias and variability in visual distance measurements made by observers during shipboard surveys of northeastern Atlantic minke whales. Reports of the International Whaling Commission 42:407-412. Schweder, T. 1996. A note on a buoy-sighting experiment in the North Sea in 1990. Reports of the International Whaling Commission 46:383-385.. 1997. Measurement error models for the Norwegian minke whale survey in 1995. Reports of the International Whaling Commission 47:485-488.

Utech, D. 1999. Valuing Hawai`i s humpback whales: the economic impact of humpbacks on Hawai`i s ocean tour boat industry. Report to the Hawaiian Islands Humpback Whale National Marine Sanctuary, Kihei, Hawaii, USA.

Table 1. Comparison of distance estimate characteristics for different treatments. Treatment 1 Variability 2 Mean/Median (SD) Accuracy 3 Mean/Median (SD) Underestimates (%) Number of measurements per treatment All females (n = 48) 35/33 (24) 78/71 (36) 77 317 All males (n = 58) 44/34 (58) 98/83 (73) 68 385 Female passengers (n = 41) 38/37 (25) 76/68 (39) 78 244 Male passengers (n = 46) 52/42 (65) 100/80 (83) 67 284 All captains (n = 6) 19/17 (15) 89/88 (22) 77 57 All naturalists (n = 13) 25/24 (15) 89/87 (27) 65 117 All passengers (n = 87) 45/39 (51) 90/72 (68) 73 528 Close to water (1-1.8 m) 43/36 (53) 95/80 (68) 67 185 High above water (5.5 m) 38/33 (44) 87/77 (57) 74 517 Surface-active whales 40/35 (49) 97/82 (54) 66 195 Non-surface-active whales 39/31 (38) 86/75 (62) 74 507 1 Sample sizes given indicate number of individuals. 2 Variability calculated as (Absolute(Measured Distance Estimated Distance)/Measured Distance)*100). 3 Accuracy (percentage of true distance) calculated as ((Estimated Distance /Measured Distance)*100).

Estimated Distance (yards) 600 500 400 300 200 100 0 Estimated=Measured Regression Line R 2 = 0.3523 0 100 200 300 400 500 600 Estimated Distance (yards) 600 500 400 300 200 100 0 Estimated=Measured Regression Line R 2 = 0.178 0 100 200 300 400 500 600 Measured Distance (yards) Figure 1. Scatter plots of measured distances versus estimated distances (both in yards) for all females (top) and all males (bottom). The line of equality (measured = estimated) is shown, as well as the best fit lines for least squares regression. In both cases distance tends to be underestimated.

600 Estimated Distance (yards) 500 400 300 200 100 Estimated=Measured Regression Line R 2 = 0.5823 0 0 100 200 300 400 500 600 600 Estimated Distance (yards) 500 400 300 200 100 Estimated=Measured Regression Line R 2 = 0.6752 0 0 100 200 300 400 500 600 Measured Distance (yards) Figure 2. Scatter plots of measured distances versus estimated distances (both in yards) for individuals with experience at distance estimation (females shown at top, males at bottom). The line of equality (measured = estimated) is shown, as well as the best fit line for a least squares regression. Direction and magnitude of bias is similar in both cases, though the fit to the regression is much stronger for experienced individuals than for inexperienced individuals (cf. Figure 1).