Acoustic and diving behavior of sperm whales (Physeter macrocephalus) during natural and depredation foraging in the Gulf of Alaska

Similar documents
Can we be as clever as a sperm whale?

Final Report Alaska Department of Fish and Game State Wildlife Grant T July 1, 2003 June 30, 2006:

Final report, OGP/JIP 22 06/01, Project 3.7.1: Testing of potential alerting sound. Aaron Thode, Delphine Mathias. Janice Straley

~ A Behavioral Response Study in 2007 &2008 (BRS 07/08) was conducted in the Bahamas to

BAJA BLUE WHALE RESEARCH 2011

Pacific Missile Range Facility (PMRF) Species Verification and Satellite Tagging Test: January Post-Test Report

Marine Mammals. James M. Price. Division of Environmental Sciences. from NOAA photograph library

Marine Mammal Acoustic Tracking from Adapting HARP Technologies

Fine-Scale Survey of Right and Humpback Whale Prey Abundance and Distribution

Fine-scale Focal DTAG Behavioral Study in the Gulf of Maine

DECEMBER 15-18, 1997 ADELAIDE, SOUTH AUSTRALIA. Douglas H. Cato. Defence Science and Technology Organisation PO Box 44, Pyrmont, NSW, 2009, Australia

Year Avg. TAC Can Others Totals

SCIENTIFIC COMMITTEE SEVENTH REGULAR SESSION August 2011 Pohnpei, Federated States of Micronesia

Real Time Localization of Sperm Whales Using a Towed Array Hydrophone

Minimal influence of wind and tidal height on underwater noise in Haro Strait

Habitat Use and Behavioral Monitoring of Hawaiian Monk Seals in Proximity to the Navy Hawaii Range Complex

BEFORE YOU OPEN ANY FILES:

Data Set 7: Bioerosion by Parrotfish Background volume of bites The question:

COSTA RICA DOME BLUE WHALE CRUISE REPORT Todd Chandler and John Calambokidis Cascadia Research Collective November 2004

Biological & Behavioral Response Studies of. Marine Mammals in Southern California, BRS.org. Photos taken under NMFS permit #

BEFORE YOU OPEN ANY FILES:

COMPARISON OF CONTEMPORANEOUS WAVE MEASUREMENTS WITH A SAAB WAVERADAR REX AND A DATAWELL DIRECTIONAL WAVERIDER BUOY

Right Whale Diving and Foraging Behavior in the Southwestern Gulf of Maine

2016 West Coast Entanglement Summary

Fine-scale Focal Dtag Behavioral Study of Diel Trends in Activity Budgets and Sound Production of Endangered Baleen Whales in the Gulf of Maine

WildCat RF unit (0.5W), full 32-byte transmissions with built-in checksum

COMPARISON OF CONTEMPORANEOUS WAVE MEASUREMENTS WITH A SAAB WAVERADAR REX AND A DATAWELL DIRECTIONAL WAVERIDER BUOY

Acoustic and Visual Survey of Cetaceans at Palmyra Atoll

SEDAR31-DW21. Examining delayed mortality in barotrauma afflicted red snapper using acoustic telemetry and hyperbaric experimentation

PUV Wave Directional Spectra How PUV Wave Analysis Works

What s UP in the. Pacific Ocean? Learning Objectives

SEA GRANT PROGRESS REPORT

2016 ANNUAL FISH TRAWL SURVEY REPORT

OR DUNGENESS CRAB FISHERY:

I. Monitoring and Adaptive Management Approach. Potential Effects, Monitoring Studies & Mitigation

Can trawling effort be identified from satellite-based VMS data?

and found that there exist a significant overlap between the billfish resources and the exploitation activities targeting tunas and mahi mahi.

National Report on Large Whale Entanglements

Wind Flow Validation Summary

Characterizing The Surf Zone With Ambient Noise Measurements

Fine Scale Baleen Whale Behavior Observed Via Tagging Over Daily Time Scales

CORRELATION BETWEEN SONAR ECHOES AND SEA BOTTOM TOPOGRAPHY

46 Chapter 8 Statistics: An Introduction

Spatial/Seasonal overlap between the midwater trawl herring fishery and predator focused user groups

COMPARISON OF DEEP-WATER ADCP AND NDBC BUOY MEASUREMENTS TO HINDCAST PARAMETERS. William R. Dally and Daniel A. Osiecki

Using DTAGs to understand sound use, behavior, and vessel and associated noise effects in Southern Resident killer whales

Chapter 5: Methods and Philosophy of Statistical Process Control

SEASONDE DETECTION OF TSUNAMI WAVES

From Clicks to Counts

7 th International Conference on Wind Turbine Noise Rotterdam 2 nd to 5 th May 2017

LIMPET tagging of Hawaiian odontocetes: assessing reproduction and estimating survival of tagged and non tagged individuals

WHAT CAN WE LEARN FROM COMPETITION ANALYSIS AT THE 1999 PAN PACIFIC SWIMMING CHAMPIONSHIPS?

Remote Monitoring of Dolphins and Whales in the High Naval Activity Areas in Hawaiian Waters

2nd Technical Research Paper Converted to Docent Friendly Summary Status. October 9, 2013

Tutorial for the. Total Vertical Uncertainty Analysis Tool in NaviModel3

Marine Mammal Scientific Support Research Programme MMSS/001/11

Calculation of Trail Usage from Counter Data

Legendre et al Appendices and Supplements, p. 1

An Application of Signal Detection Theory for Understanding Driver Behavior at Highway-Rail Grade Crossings

Nortek Technical Note No.: TN-021. Chesapeake Bay AWAC Evaluation

GPS/TDR Satellite Tracking of Sperm Whales with 3-axis Accelerometers- Background. Prepared by Oregon State University.

Near-Field Sturgeon Monitoring for the New NY Bridge at Tappan Zee. Quarterly Report July 1 September 30, 2014

Analyses of the Scoring of Writing Essays For the Pennsylvania System of Student Assessment

Movements of satellite-tagged false killer whales around the main Hawaiian Islands: implications for management

Evaluating the Influence of R3 Treatments on Fishing License Sales in Pennsylvania

VOCALIZATION AND COORDINATED FEEDING BEHAVIOR OF THE HUMPBACK WHALE IN SOUTHEASTERN ALASKA

Right Whale Diving and Foraging Behavior in the Southwestern Gulf of Maine

Preliminary analysis of yellowfin tuna catch, effort, size and tagging data using an integrated age-structured model

Pelagic fishery for Sebastes mentella in the Irminger Sea

AIS data analysis for vessel behavior during strong currents and during encounters in the Botlek area in the Port of Rotterdam

DEPARTMENT OF FISH AND GAME

Description of Underwater Noise Attenuation System Design Unit 2. New NY Bridge Project

Acoustic Seaglider for Beaked Whale Detection

Hydroacoustic surveys of Otsego Lake s pelagic fish community,

Evaluation of the Klein HydroChart 3500 Interferometric Bathymetry Sonar for NOAA Sea Floor Mapping

BOTTOM MAPPING WITH EM1002 /EM300 /TOPAS Calibration of the Simrad EM300 and EM1002 Multibeam Echo Sounders in the Langryggene calibration area.

Preliminary results of SEPODYM application to albacore. in the Pacific Ocean. Patrick Lehodey

Winter Steelhead Redd to Fish conversions, Spawning Ground Survey Data

Status of humpback, blue, and gray whales along the US West Coast and relevance to entanglements

OCN 201 Lab Fall 2009 OCN 201. Lab 9 - El Niño

Currents measurements in the coast of Montevideo, Uruguay

Review on Size Sampling Frameworks for North Atlantic Albacore of Taiwanese Logline Fleets

Map Showing NAFO Management Units

Palythoa Abundance and Coverage in Relation to Depth

DISTRIBUTION STATEMENT A. Approved for public release; distribution is unlimited.

Patterns of Southern Resident Killer Whale (Orcinus orca) Movement in Relation to the Tides and Currents

Conservation Limits and Management Targets

A Combined Recruitment Index for Demersal Juvenile Cod in NAFO Divisions 3K and 3L

5.1 Introduction. Learning Objectives

Homework 2a Bathymetric Charts [based on the Chauffe & Jefferies (2007)]

Underwater noise and offshore windfarms

Long-term killer whale monitoring in Prince William Sound/ Kenai Fjords

Discover the Gray Whale

ARTICLE IN PRESS. Deep-Sea Research I

Waves, Bubbles, Noise, and Underwater Communications

APPENDIX 3E HYDROACOUSTIC ANALYSIS OF FISH POPULATIONS IN COPCO AND IRON GATE RESERVOIRS, CALIFORNIA NOVEMBER

Spatial Methods for Road Course Measurement

Assessment Summary Report Gulf of Mexico Red Snapper SEDAR 7

Appendix 4-1 Seasonal Acoustic Monitoring Study on Oahu Army Installations C. Pinzari USGS 2014

U.S. National Observer Program, Southeast Regional Fishery Observer Programs & Regional Electronic Technology Implementation Plans Jane DiCosimo

Transcription:

Acoustic and diving behavior of sperm whales (Physeter macrocephalus) during natural and depredation foraging in the Gulf of Alaska Delphine Mathias a) and Aaron M. Thode Marine Physical Laboratory, Scripps Institution of Oceanography, University of California San Diego, La Jolla, California 92093-0238 Jan Straley University of Alaska Southeast, Sitka, Alaska 99835 John Calambokidis and Gregory S. Schorr Cascadia Research Collective, Olympia, Washington 98501 Kendall Folkert P.O. Box 6497, Sitka, Alaska 99835 (Received 13 September 2011; revised 25 April 2012; accepted 2 May 2012) Sperm whales have depredated black cod (Anoplopoma fimbria) from demersal longlines in the Gulf of Alaska for decades, but the behavior has recently spread in intensity and geographic coverage. Over a three-year period 11 bioacoustic tags were attached to adult sperm whales off Southeast Alaska during both natural and depredation foraging conditions. Measurements of the animals dive profiles and their acoustic behavior under both behavioral modes were examined for statistically significant differences. Two rough categories of depredation are identified: deep and shallow. Deep depredating whales consistently surface within 500 m of a hauling fishing vessel, have maximum dive depths greater than 200 m, and display significantly different acoustic behavior than naturally foraging whales, with shorter inter-click intervals, occasional bouts of high creak rates, and fewer dives without creaks. Shallow depredating whales conduct dives that are much shorter, shallower, and more acoustically active than both the natural and deep depredating behaviors, with median creak rates three times that of natural levels. These results suggest that depredation efforts might be measured remotely with passive acoustic monitoring at close ranges. VC 2012 Acoustical Society of America. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1121/1.4726005] PACS number(s): 43.80.Ka, 43.80.Nd [WWA] Pages: 518 532 I. INTRODUCTION A. Background on sperm whale foraging and acoustic behavior Sperm whales (Physeter macrocephalus) are a cosmopolitan species distributed throughout the world s oceans (Whitehead, 2003; Berzin, 1971). While females and immature individuals generally reside at low latitudes, adult males also travel and forage at higher latitudes (Whitehead et al., 1992; Teloni et al., 2007). In the U.S., these whales are listed as an endangered species, but their current population in the North Pacific is unknown. A deep-diving species, sperm whales regularly descend to depths greater than 400 m for periods ranging between 30 and 45 min, and rest at the surface for periods ranging between 5 and 10 min (Papastavrou et al., 1989; Jaquet et al., 2001; Wahlberg, 2002; Watwood et al., 2006). The few data available from higher latitudes indicate shallower dive depths than what is measured in temperate or tropical latitudes (Whitehead et al., 1992; Teloni et al., 2007). a) Author to whom correspondence should be addressed. Electronic mail: delphine.mathias@gmail.com Sperm whales are vocally active underwater, and during a single dive an individual can generate thousands of impulsive sounds, called clicks (Goold et al., 1995; Worthington and Schevill, 1957; Madsen et al., 2002a; Wahlberg, 2002). Measurements in other regions of the world indicate that a whale typically falls silent about 10 to 15 min before it returns to the surface (Madsen et al., 2002a; Douglas et al., 2005; Watwood et al., 2006), so by passively monitoring an animal s vocalizations, its dive cycle can be estimated. In the Gulf of Alaska (GOA), click sounds from sperm whales have been detected throughout the year on bottom-mounted recorders, revealing a year-long presence in the region (Mellinger et al., 2004). Another distinctive acoustic feature of sperm whales is the existence of creak (or buzz ) sounds, a sequence of pulses produced at a rate of 10 per second or faster (Madsen et al., 2002), and often characterized by a decrease in the pulse interval and (occasionally) amplitude over the 5 10 s duration of the sound (Whitehead and Weilgart, 1990; Whitehead, 2003). Teloni et al. (2007) and Miller et al. (2004) reported creak rates close to 15 creaks per hour. Watwood et al. (2006) reported that 37 sperm whales in the Atlantic Ocean, the Gulf of Mexico and the Ligurian Sea made 22 6 8.0 creaks per hour. Previous bioacoustic tagging 518 J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 132 (1), July 2012 0001-4966/2012/132(1)/518/15/$30.00 VC 2012 Acoustical Society of America

work on sperm whales has shown that most creaks occur at foraging depths and are often associated with changes in the orientation of the animal (Miller et al., 2004; Watwood et al., 2006). Creaks are often followed by a few seconds of silence before the animal resumes usual clicking (Madsen et al., 2002), defined here as a creak-pause event. There are several possible interpretations of creak-pause events. One is that these intervals are used to recycle air within the sound production system (Wahlberg et al., 2002). Because creaks generally have lower received levels on hydrophones than usual clicks, it may also be possible that some clicks at the end of a creak become masked by noise, creating a false impression of silence. Another unproved hypothesis is that the silences are indicative of prey capture. Analogous signals observed in bats, dolphins, and beaked whales suggest that creaks are echolocation signals (Gordon, 1987; Madsen et al., 2002), and periods of time where creaks are detected have been described as prey capture attempts (Miller et al.,2004; Watwood et al.,2006). Laboratory studies on bat echolocation have found that post-buzz pause durations were longer after successful captures (Surlykke et al., 2003) than after unsuccessful attempts, but extending this conclusion to cetaceans is risky, as bats cannot produce sound when ingesting prey, while at least one cetacean species can (DeRuiter et al., 2009). Miller et al. (2004) found that the majority of creaks produced by sperm whales in the Ligurian Sea and the Gulf of Mexico are followed by pauses of about 5 s. Beaked whales and porpoises often pause their echolocation for less than 2 s (Johnson et al., 2004, DeRuiter et al., 2009) and DeRuiter et al. (2009) found that porpoises ended creaks 0.8 s after prey capture, even though the animals are physiologically capable of echolocating and ingesting prey simultaneously. If this hypothesis is valid, then the presence of silences following creaks may permit the discrimination of successful prey capture attempts from unsuccessful ones on the basis of acoustic data obtained within a few kilometer range. Thus the duration of creaks, the rate at which they are produced, and the relative fraction of creaks that are followed by silence are all variables of interest in characterizing sperm whale acoustic dive behavior. The diet of sperm whales generally consists of various cephalopod species, based on analyses of stomach contents (Okutani and Nemoto, 1964; Kawakami, 1980; Evans and Hindell, 2004). However, in certain regions fish seem to comprise an unknown fraction of the diet as well (Clarke, 1980; Kawakami, 1980; Whitehead, 2003), including the eastern Gulf of Alaska (Kawakami, 1980). B. Sperm whale depredation The question of the relative importance of fish to the sperm whale diet is a matter of practical concern, because sperm whales are known to take fish from fishing gear, a behavior known as depredation. Although quantitative data are limited, sperm whale depredation appears to be increasing worldwide (Ashford et al.,1996; Capdeville, 1997; Nolan and Liddle, 2000; Purves et al., 2004). To date reports have been received from fishermen, engineers, fishery observers and biologists from Norway, Greenland, eastern Canada (Labrador and Newfoundland) and the Falkland Islands regarding interactions with sperm whales and bottlenose whales (Newfoundland only) in longline fisheries off these countries. Perez et al. (2006) estimated that marine mammal depredation on the combined longline fisheries in Alaska caused a loss of about 2.2% of the total fishery groundfish catch during 1998 2004, based on visual evidence of torn or partial fish. In the eastern Gulf of Alaska (GOA) a demersal longline fishery for sablefish (Anoplopoma fimbria) occurs about 8.5 months a year. Sablefish (also called blackcod and butterfish) reside on the continental slope, and most commercial longliners operate in water depths between 400 and 1000 m. The continental shelf off Kruzof, Baranof, and Chichagof islands, located near Sitka, AK, is very narrow; consequently, the sablefish grounds are within 12 miles of shore (see Fig. 1). A domestic sablefish survey in the GOA looked at catch rates from 1999 2001 for all sets with sperm whales present; they compared sets with and without physical evidence of depredation and found a 5% lower catch rate in sets with depredation evidence (Sigler, 2008). In 2003 the Southeast Alaska Sperm Whale Avoidance Project (SEASWAP) was created to investigate this issue with the long-term goal of reducing depredation. A collaborative study between fishermen, scientists and managers, SEASWAP works with the coastal fishing fleet to collect various quantitative data on longline depredation. Using the shape of the flukes as a unique identifier, SEASWAP found that at least 106 individual sperm whales have been involved in depredation. Bayesian mark-recapture analyses estimate at least 123 ([94 174]; 95% credible interval) depredating whales in the GOA study area (Thode et al., 2006). The most ambitious SEASWAP field effort deployed eleven bioacoustic tags on sperm whales in 2007 and 2009, under both natural and depredation conditions off the continental shelf of Sitka, AK. The archival tags logged acoustic, depth, and orientation data. In this paper these tag records are analyzed to address three issues. First, do natural foraging dives in Alaska differ from those measured from populations that rely primarily on cephalopods for their diet? Second, do acoustic behavioral parameters change significantly when the animals are depredating? To our knowledge few to no depredating marine mammals have ever been tagged before, and so the strategies employed by the animals are of interest, perhaps even suggesting mechanisms for reducing this behavior or its impact on the fishery. Finally, can passive acoustic monitoring be used to quantify depredation rates? Current methods rely on visual evidence at the surface, which is known to undercount depredation rates. A better measuring stick of depredation effort and/or success would be useful for evaluating the efficacy of future countermeasures. II. MATERIALS AND METHODS A. Equipment The acoustic behavior, dive profiles, and spatial orientation of sperm whales were investigated using digital acoustic B-probe sampling tags (Burgess et al., 1998; Goldbogen et al., 2006; Johnson et al., 2009). Besides sampling acoustic J. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 132, No. 1, July 2012 Mathias et al.: Acoustic behavior of depredating sperm whales 519

FIG. 1. (Color online) Local bathymetry off Sitka, AK, marked with fishing haul positions for the two tagging examples discussed in detail in Secs. III B and III C. Water depths are shown in meters. data, the B-probe contains a pressure sensor and a two-axis accelerometer (MXA2500GL, Memsic, Inc., North Andover, MA 01845) with one axis parallel to the longitudinal axis of the probe. Data from the depth gauge and accelerometers are sampled at 1 Hz and stored within the tag. The acoustic signal was passed through a Linear Technologies LTC1164-6 switched-capacitor elliptic anti-aliasing filter before entering the analog-digital converter. The acoustic data analyzed in this paper were sampled at 4096 Hz, sufficient for detecting regular clicks and creaks. B. Deployments and visual observation protocols The SEASWAP tagging effort used a 16 foot rigidhulled inflatable boat (RHIB), which mostly operated in the vicinity of cooperating fishing vessels in order to spot tagging opportunities. The two fishing vessels discussed in this paper include a small commercial fishing vessel (F/V Cobra, 18 m), and a larger vessel (F/V Ocean Prowler, 38 m) performing the NOAA Southeast Alaska sablefish survey. No other fishing vessels were within 15 miles of either vessel during the tagging studies discussed here, as confirmed by both the RHIB visual observations and radar monitoring by the F/V Cobra. The tag was deployed from the RHIB using a modified windsurfing mast. Photographs were taken of the relative orientation of the tag on the animal. Once tagged, a whale was identified and followed via both visual sighting and monitoring the tag radio beacon. Whenever possible, further photographs of the tag placement were taken to note whether the relative orientation of the tag on the whale had shifted over time. No significant shifting of tag position was ever observed. The tagging team was active mostly in the early morning, with the goal of deploying tags on animals before the start of a fishing haul. Once a haul began the tagging boat drifted away from the fishing vessel to avoid unduly influencing animal behavior. The visual observers noted times and distances of surfacing animals relative to the vessel, recorded subsequent orientation and surface movements, and noted times of fluke ups, indicative of the start of a foraging dive. Photos were taken of most of the individuals surfacing within 500 m of the vessel. Individuals could then be identified by photo-id, due to the presence of distinctive profiles, scars, and coloring on all sides of the whale. Distances were estimated by a laser range-finder, when possible; otherwise, the range was marked as being greater than or less than 500 m range from the vessel. Observers each day calibrated their visual distance determinations of 500 m with the laser range finder. The presence of an animal within 500 m of a vessel was used to help classify whether a whale was in a depredation or natural foraging state, for reasons to be described in Sec. II E. C. Dive profile analysis The pressure sensor data on the B-probe tags permitted recovery of dive profiles. The beginning and end of a given dive are defined as times when the animal s depth became 520 J. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 132, No. 1, July 2012 Mathias et al.: Acoustic behavior of depredating sperm whales

deeper or shallower than 10 m. Within each dive, a set of dive inflections are defined as points where the vertical velocity of the whale (the time derivative of the pressure) changed sign, consistent with the definitions used in Miller et al. (2004). After an inflection is noted, an ensuing net vertical change of at least 10 m (approximately 2/3 of a typical body length) was required before a new inflection could be flagged. The number of inflections logged during each dive was normalized by dividing the number of inflections in a dive by the total dive duration, yielding a rate of dive inflections per hour, or Infl. The surface, bottom, and total dive durations (T S ; T b ; T d ), as well as the maximum depth attained (D max ) were also extracted from each dive. The bottom time is measured from the end of the descent phase (the time when the pitch of the diving whale first rises above the horizontal plane) to the start of the ascent phase (the last point in time when the animal s pitch is below the horizontal plane), as defined in Miller et al. (2004). D. Acoustic data analysis Sperm whale regular clicks were automatically detected in the acoustic tag data by generating a series of 256 pt FFTs, overlapped 75%, and then integrating the power spectral density between 1200 and 1900 Hz. If a value exceeded a running-average estimate of background noise level by 20 db, the presence of a click was flagged; otherwise, the information was used to update the running average. The output of this automated click detector was manually spot-checked to confirm that clicks produced by other nearby non-tagged whales were not incorporated into the results. Detecting creaks was more difficult, because their signal-to-noise ratio (SNR) is generally much lower. Creak sounds are almost always preceded by a set of regular clicks with steadily decreasing inter-click intervals (ICI), which eventually transition into the creak. During a creak the ICI decreases from 0.2 to 0.02 s (Gordon, 1987) and the creak amplitude decreases over time, with clicks at the end of a creak often 20 db or more lower in level than at the beginning (Madsen et al., 2002). Creak detection was semi-automated. The first step used automated click processing to note gaps in regular click trains, where a gap is defined as any pause between detected clicks between 5 and 60 s duration. Each gap was reviewed manually and aurally for the presence of a creak, and then categorized as a silence, creak-only, or creak-pause event. After a creak-only event, the whale began producing regular clicks within two seconds after the audible end of a creak, while creak-pause events contained at least two seconds of silence between the end of an audible creak and the resumption of a click train. Special efforts were made to ensure that no creaks were missed due to the relatively low acoustic sampling rate (4096 Hz) of the tag. Whenever a gap was first categorized as silence but preceded by a decrease in the ICI of a regular click sequence, the sample was re-reviewed aurally and usually categorized as creak-only or creak-pause. Of the silent gaps generated by the automated detector that were preceded by a decrease in the regular click ICI, only 2% provided no aural evidence of a creak on further examination, and were thus categorized as true silence. Therefore, a steady decrease in the ICI of usual clicks proved to be a reliable indicator of an upcoming creak event. To address the possibility that extended pauses after creaks arose from noise masking weak creaks, the duration of each creak was also estimated. The start time of the creak was defined as the when the ICI dropped below 0.2 s, and the end time was defined as the start of a silent period of 2 s or longer duration. Every tag record was decomposed into a set of dive profiles, with the beginning and end of each dive defined according to the criteria of the previous section. The following acoustic parameters were then extracted from each dive. (a) Timing of first click (T Cl1 ): the time difference in minutes between the start of a dive and when the first click is detected on the tag. (b) Click rate (Cl): the total number of clicks produced during a dive, divided by the total dive duration in seconds. Clicks associated with creaks were excluded from the analysis. While in principle heavy bouts of creaking could also reduce this value, in reality creak rates were never high enough to reduce the click rate. (c) Mean inter-click-interval (ICI): the mean interval in seconds between successive clicks within the same click train. Creak clicks (with intervals less than 0.2 s) were excluded from this calculation. Note that ICI can differ from Cl if the whale is silent during substantial (d) portions of the dive. Normalized creak-only (Cr) and creak-pause (CrP) rates: the number of a particular type of creak event produced during a dive, divided by the total dive duration in seconds. (e) Fraction of creak-only (F Cr ) and creak-pause (F CrP ) events: the relative proportion of each creak event category for each dive. E. Behavioral categories and hypothesis testing The tag records were divided into four distinct behavioral categories, using the B-probe dive profile records, fluke photographs of tagged whales, and visual observations of sperm whale surface behavior. No acoustic or dive parameters (other than maximum dive depth) were used to assign a given dive to a category. The categories are defined as follows. (1) Resting: behaviors characterized by shallow drifting and minimal changes in body orientation, regardless of whether a fishing vessel is hauling. (2) Natural foraging: dives were conducted by animals at least 500 m from an actively hauling fishing vessel, or during times when the fishing vessel was not hauling gear. The distribution of estimated ranges of surfacing animals (using a laser-range finder or estimated visually as described in Sec. II B) generally clustered around 400 m or less, or at distances much greater than 500 m. A visual J. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 132, No. 1, July 2012 Mathias et al.: Acoustic behavior of depredating sperm whales 521

range of 500 m was thus chosen to demarcate the natural and depredation categories. If a tagged whale was visually identified consistently surfacing within 500 m of a hauling fishing vessel, then the subsequent dive was assigned to one of the following two categories. (3) Deep depredation: Maximum dive depths are greater than 200 m. (4) Shallow depredation: Maximum dive depths are shallower than 200 m. A potential objection to this categorization scheme is that fishing vessels and naturally foraging whales might exploit the same productive locations, and thus these whales could randomly surface within 500 m of a hauling vessel. There is indeed evidence that depredation is more statistically likely to occur at more productive fishing spots (Straley, 2009); however, it is unlikely whales are surfacing close to the vessel by coincidence, for several reasons. First, experience has shown that sperm whales will generally not surface within 500 m of a fishing vessel before or after a haul, even if the vessel drifts for hours at the location of a fishing set. Just after a haul ends, a given whale will sometimes be sighted for one or two dives within 500 m of the vessel, but the range of the visual sightings always increases over time, unless the fishing vessel starts to maneuver or display other actions that may be associated with the beginning a of second haul. Second, a hauling vessel generally travels 4 6 km laterally across the ocean floor when recovering longline gear, sometimes against the local current, but the same whales are consistently sighted within 500 m of the vessel during the entire haul. Thus the animals are diving in reference to the moving platform, and not in reference to some particular bathymetric feature, as might be expected were the whales simply exploiting a local hot spot of productivity. Finally, the surface behavior of animals within 500 m of a hauling vessel is markedly different than that observed from a RHIB without a fishing vessel present. During a haul animals at the surface often orient their heads to point directly toward the vessel, often dipping their heads a slight amount underwater, while simultaneously generating bursts of clicks intense enough to be heard by a listener 1 m above the ocean surface. The cumulative impression left by these visual observations is that a whale consistently surfacing within 500 m of a hauling vessel should be assigned a different behavioral category than a whale surfacing during times when a haul is not occurring. The specific hypothesis examined in this paper is whether statistically significant differences in acoustic and diving behavior can be gleaned between categories initially defined by visual observations, vessel state, and maximum dive depth. The distributions of the dive and acoustic parameters obtained for each category are non-gaussian, often highly skewed, and characterized by large tails that indicate relatively infrequent but significant events that could not be discounted as outliers. Thus a two-sided Kolmogorov Smirnov (KS) test was used to evaluate the probability that two sample parameter distributions, obtained from different behavioral categories, could have been drawn from the same underlying cumulative probability distribution. The null hypothesis is that various parameters measured from both behavioral categories were drawn from the same underlying distribution. A Bonferroni correction is applied to account for the number of dependent or independent statistical tests performed (Bonferroni, 1936). As ten parameters are tested for significant differences between behavioral categories, the p value for an individual test is reduced to 0.05/10 ¼ 0.005. Thus, KS p-values of less than 0.005 led to the rejection of the null hypothesis at the 5% significance level. Two KS tests were performed for each parameter of interest. The first test used all dives assigned to a given category, regardless of what individual produced the dive. The second test attempted to account for potential differences in individual behavior by only using tag records that contained both natural and depredation behaviors. The test then divides natural foraging dives into two categories: those occurring on a tag record associated with deep depredation states and those associated with shallow depredation states. No single tag record displayed both deep and shallow depredation states, so no ambiguity existed when assigning a dive to a category. The parameter distributions for shallow and deep depredating individuals are then compared with their associated natural foraging parameter distributions using the KS test. Differences in sample sizes between the categories are automatically incorporated into the two-sided KS test. A potential objection to this method is that the data itself is used to define behavioral categories, so there could be a risk of correlation between the categories and the parameters tested for significant differences between them. However, only the distance between the fishing boat and a whale is used to define the whale state as natural behavior or depredation behavior. Only the maximum depth of the whale is used to separate deep depredation and shallow depredation behaviors. All other dive and acoustic parameters are not used to define the categories. III. RESULTS A. Summary of tag records Acoustic tags were deployed on sperm whales during two field efforts in July 2007 and June 2009. In 2007 eight B-probes recorded a total of 79 h of animal depth, orientation, and acoustic data. In 2009 three B-probes were deployed, generating 67 h of animal depth, orientation, and acoustic data. Thus over the course of two field seasons, seven distinct animals were successfully tagged 11 times with functioning B-probes, and all were matched with a photo-identification catalog. Two individuals were tagged twice in 2007, and one individual was tagged in both 2007 and 2009. The mean, median, and mode of the tag deployment times were 9.8, 7.0, and 9.0 h in 2007 and 22.3, 27.0, and 12.0 h in 2009. A given tag record from a long deployment could contain several different behavioral states, and in the next two subsections two such tag records are reviewed in detail, collectively illustrating the four states defined in Sec. II E. Eight tag records had periods of natural foraging, documenting a total of 135 natural dives. Ten of the 11 whales tagged 522 J. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 132, No. 1, July 2012 Mathias et al.: Acoustic behavior of depredating sperm whales

showed some degree of vessel association, in that the tagged animal consistently surfaced within 500 m of a hauling vessel. Seven of the ten tagged whales seen surfacing close to a longline fishing vessel displayed deep depredation behaviors, while the other three displayed shallow depredation behaviors. A total of 52 deep depredation dives and 35 shallow depredation dives were documented. Whales displaying shallow depredation also conducted a total of 64 natural dives, while the deep depredating whales also cumulatively displayed 66 natural dives. Three tags displayed 23 deep depredation dives with no natural foraging activity, and so these dives were excluded from the second KS test. The statistical differences between the diving and acoustic parameters of the behavioral categories, excluding resting behavior, are presented in Sec. III D. B. Resting, natural foraging, and deep depredation behaviors from a tag deployed on 12 June 2009 This tag record is among the longest available (28 hours of data), and covers two complete fishing hauls. The whale displaying this tag record had been following the F/V Cobra since 11 June 2009, before being tagged close to the vessel at 13:53 on 12 June 2009. Subsequently the first longline haul began at 14:00 and ended four hours later. The vessel began its second haul the following day (13 June) at 11:15, finishing at 14:30. Visual observers sighted three whales during the first haul and six whales during the second haul, all consistently surfacing within 400 m of the vessel. Figure 2 displays the whale s dive profiles, along with each dive s assigned behavioral category, inflection rate, click rate, mean ICI, and total creak rate (both with and without subsequent pauses). With the exception of the maximum dive depth, none of the data shown in Fig. 2 were used to assign a dive to a particular behavioral category. The start and end of the fishing hauls are indicated by the solid and dotted vertical lines, respectively. A shaded area indicates that the tagged whale was sighted surfacing within 400 m of the fishing vessel for each shaded dive. The tag detached around 18:00 on 13 June. At various times this whale displayed three of the four behavioral states defined in Sec. II E. The labeled horizontal bars in Fig. 2 indicate each state. (1) Resting between 14:30 and 15:20 on 13 June, after completion of the second haul: the animal remains at less than 30 m depth, and its inflection rate, creak rate, and click rate are at levels much lower than natural foraging conditions. (2) Natural foraging behavior between 18:00 on 12 June and 11:25 on 13 June, and between 15:20 and 18:00 on 13 June: the animal shows considerable variation in dive depth, with the dive profiles systematically shallowing and deepening between 250 and 750 m throughout the night and morning. The normalized creak rate also varies between 5 and 20 creaks/h. During the natural foraging state 54% of creaks detected (F CrP )were followed by distinctive pauses. (3) Deep depredation during both hauls: during the first haul, all depth and acoustic behaviors displayed by the animal lie within the range of normal foraging behavior, with the exception of a high creak rate of over 30 creaks/h for one dive. During this phase 43% of detected creaks were followed by distinctive pauses. During the second haul, the animal s depth range and usual click parameters also lay within normal bounds; however, the dive inflection rate is slightly greater than average, and the creak rate attains or exceeds 30 creaks/h through half the haul, then drops off to nothing for one dive. Only 30% of creaks were followed by distinctive pauses. The ICI seems similar during both natural and depredation states. FIG. 2. (Color online) Selected dive and acoustic parameters of a whale displaying resting, natural foraging behavior and deep depredation behavior on 12 June 2009: (a) dive profile; (b) normalized dive inflection rate per hour, Infl, with each bar representing a distinct dive; (c) click rate per second, Cl; (d) mean inter-clickinterval ICI per dive, with dotted lines corresponding to one standard deviation; and (e) normalized creak rate per hour, combining creakonly and creak-pause events. The start and end times of the fishing hauls are indicated by the solid and dotted vertical lines, respectively. The shaded areas indicate when the tagged whale was visually sighted surfacing within 400 m of the F/V Cobra for every dive. J. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 132, No. 1, July 2012 Mathias et al.: Acoustic behavior of depredating sperm whales 523

The water depth at both haul locations was 720 m, so the tagged whale sometimes dove all the way down to the ocean floor during deep depredation. The timing of dive inflections and timing of creaks is highly correlated, with a correlation coefficient of 0.78. C. Natural foraging behavior and shallow depredation from a tag deployed on 17 July 2007 On 17 July 2007 a whale was tagged at 7:50, close to the F/V Ocean Prowler, which was deploying two longline sets at 690 m water depth. The vessel began hauling its first longline set at 10:00, ending at 12:50, and then began hauling its second longline set at 13:30, ending at 18:00. A total of four whales surfaced within 500 m of the Prowler throughout both hauls, with the tagged whale consistently surfacing between 150 and 400 m astern of the Prowler during the hauls. After 17:30 the whale was sighted swimming away from the vessel, and by the end of the haul it was at least 2 km away. Figure 3 displays the whale s dive statistics in a format identical to that of Fig. 2. Between 7:50 and 10:00 the whale s dives became gradually shallower and less regular in terms of surface and dive durations. During both hauls, the whale performed shallow and irregular dives (120 6 83 m). Between the end of the first haul and the start of the second, the whale made a 37 min dive down to 510 m. At 17:00, 30 min before the end of the second haul, the whale s dives became deeper and more regular in terms of surface and dive duration. From 18:00 until the tag released the whale alternated 28 min dives down to a median depth of 350 m with 11 min surface times. During the shallow depredation state multiple dive parameters show significant differences from the other behavioral states. For example, during the haul depths are shallow (less than 200 m) compared to the remainder of the tag record (deeper than 300 m). During shallow depredation inflection rates are higher by a factor of 2, click rates are six times higher, the mean ICI is smaller, and creak rates are 10 times higher. During shallow depredation 65% of all creaks were subsequently followed by silence (i.e., creak-pause events). Only three natural dives before and after the haul displayed creak sounds, with a creak-pause fraction of 47%, and click rates are very low, with minutes of time elapsing without the animal making a sound at depth. The timing of dive inflections and timing of creaks had a correlation coefficient of 0.71. D. Comparison of behavioral states across all tag records Figures 4 and 5 display the dive and acoustic parameter distributions compiled from all 11 tag records. Each figure contains five subplots, corresponding to the parameters defined in Secs. II C and II D. Each subplot shows three box plots corresponding to the natural foraging, deep depredation, and shallow depredation categories. Figure 5(e) also displays the creak-pause fractions measured by other biotagging studies in temperate latitudes (Miller et al., 2004). Tables I and II summarize the mean and standard deviations of these distributions, along with the p values of the two-sample KS nonparametric tests. p-values less than 0.005 are italicized. Figures 6 and 7 use the same format as Figs. 4 and 5, but here only data from tag records that display both natural and depredation behaviors are used. Two box plots for natural foraging behavior are now shown: one for dives preceding or following deep depredation, and one for dives preceding or following shallow depredation. The box plots thus show distributions drawn from the same pools of individuals, thus reducing potential individual variations in dive or acoustic behavior. Differences in sample sizes between the categories are automatically incorporated into the two-sided KS test. FIG. 3. (Color online) Tag parameters of a whale displaying natural foraging and shallow depredation behavior on 17 July 2007, using the same format as Fig. 2. The shaded area indicates when the surfacing whale was visually sighted within 400 m of the F/V Prowler. 524 J. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 132, No. 1, July 2012 Mathias et al.: Acoustic behavior of depredating sperm whales

FIG. 4. (Color online) Boxplots of five dive parameter distributions, obtained from all tag records, subdivided by behavioral state: (a) surface duration (min); (b) dive duration (min); (c) bottom duration (min); (d) maximum dive depth (m); and (e) normalized dive inflection rate per hour. These parameters are defined in Sec. II C. The box plots show the 5th, 25th, 50th, 75th, and 95th percentile values of the distributions. Tables III and IV display the results of the KS analysis on the distributions shown in Figs. 6 and 7, with the p-values indicating the result of comparing a particular depredation category with natural foraging dives preceding or following that type of depredation. IV. DISCUSSION A. Is natural foraging behavior off Sitka similar to elsewhere in the world? A distinctive feature of sperm whale behavior is the depths of their foraging dives. Although aspects of the diving, acoustic and body motion behavior of male sperm whales have been studied at both low (Gordon, 1987; Whitehead et al., 1991; Watwood et al., 2006) and high latitudes (Whitehead et al., 1992; Jaquet et al., 2001; Madsen et al., 2002; Douglas et al. 2005; Teloni et al., 2007), no data have been published for the Gulf of Alaska area. A question that arises is whether dive and acoustic parameters of the Alaskan whales under natural foraging conditions are consistent with measurements obtained elsewhere. Male sperm whales in this study dove to mean maximum depths of 438 m (Table I) whendisplaying natural foraging behavior, with maximum dive depths of around 800 m. Thus most dives did not descend to the ocean floor, and the observed dive and surface durations [Figs. 4(a) and 4(b)] are similar to the ones reported in another FIG. 5. (Color online) Boxplots of five acoustic parameter distributions, obtained from all tag records, subdivided by behavioral state: (a) time of first click produced (min), relative to start of dive; (b) click rate per s; (c) inter-click interval (s); (d) normalized creak rate per hour (for all creak events); and (e) creak-pause percentage. These parameters are defined in Sec. II D. The box plots show the 5th, 25th, 50th, 75th, and 95th percentile values of the distributions. J. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 132, No. 1, July 2012 Mathias et al.: Acoustic behavior of depredating sperm whales 525

TABLE I. Dive statistics for 2007 and 2009 data. Values are means þ/ s.d. over the number of dives N d considered. N tag corresponds to the number of tag records displaying each behavior. N ind corresponds to the number of individuals displaying each behavior. A p-value is the probability that the distribution is drawn from the same distribution as the natural foraging distribution. Italic p-values indicate the rejection of the null-hypothesis of a common underlying distribution (p < 0.005). D max (m) T s (min) T d (min) T b (min) Infl (h 1 ) N d N tag N ind Resting 31.7 þ/ 3.5 6.1 þ/ 0.6 15.9 þ/ 2.3 7.1 þ/ 1.6 5.0 þ/ 1.8 10 3 3 Natural foraging 438.5 þ/ 135.0 8.9 þ/ 2.2 33.0 þ/ 7.9 14.5 þ/ 5.9 15.2 þ/ 8.2 135 8 6 Deep depredation 487.2 þ/ 160.4 7.4 þ/ 2.4 28.4 þ/ 9.4 11.7 þ/ 6.2 19.1 þ/ 10.8 52 7 5 p ¼ 0.02 p < 0.005 p 0.005 p < 0.005 p ¼ 0.03 Shallow depredation 145.1 þ/ 108.2 3.9 þ/ 2.6 13.8 þ/ 8.9 7.1 þ/ 5.0 35.1 þ/ 16.9 35 3 3 p 0.005 p 0.005 p 0.005 p 0.005 p 0.005 high-latitude study (Teloni et al., 2007). That same study also reported natural dives shallower than 200 m off Norway, but there has been no indication that sperm whales in this study naturally perform such shallow dives. In our study area in the eastern Gulf of Alaska, off Sitka, naturally foraging male sperm whales spent 85% of their total dive time emitting usual clicks, generally starting within 1 min of the descent phase and stopping early during the ascent phase. The mean depths of their first creaks were shallower (80 m) than the mean depths of the last creaks (300 m), suggesting that the whales were not searching for prey items during the ascent phase. These results are similar to what has been reported in other studies (Madsen et al., 2002; Douglas et al., 2005; Watwood et al., 2006). The click and creak rates, as well as the ICI values, reported in Figs. 5(b) 5(d) lie within the ranges reported by Miller et al. (2004) at low latitudes and by Teloni et al. (2007) off Norway. Miller et al. reported that while foraging at depth, 22 sperm whales from the Ligurian Sea and the Gulf of Mexico made 12.5 6 4.0 dive inflections per hour, and a mean of 32% of all creaks were produced within 10 s of a dive inflection. The sperm whales in this study made 15.2 6 8.2 dive inflections per hour [Fig. 4(e)], and consistent relationships were also found between the timing of dive inflections and creak events: 85% of creaks started within 30 s of a dive inflection, and 56% started within 10 s of a dive inflection. Three of the 11 tagged whales displayed a total of 10 resting dives, all of them being head-down drift-dives. The durations of those resting dives (15.9 6 2.3 min) and the depths at which they occurred (31.7 6 3.5 m) are consistent with those reported in Miller et al. (2008). Thus dive parameters from naturally foraging sperm whales in the eastern Gulf of Alaska are within the bounds measured at other regions around the world. However, two significant differences in natural acoustic behavior were found. First, 50% of dives were not associated with any creaks [Fig. 5(d)], which is higher than what Teloni et al. (2007) reported (15%). Second, the relative proportion of creak-pause events (F CrP ) is 48.4% 6 10.2 in Fig. 5(e), which is considerably lower than the only other published description of this fraction (88.9% 6 13.5), in Miller et al. (2004). The potential relevance of these differences is examined in Sec. IV B 3. B. Depredation vs natural foraging behavior 1. Dive parameters during shallow depredation Three of the ten tagged whales seen surfacing close to a hauling longline fishing vessel had dives assigned to the shallow depredation category. Figures 4 and 6 demonstrate how this category is substantially different from the other categories across all dive parameters. The maximum dive depths of animals during this state were generally much shallower than those of naturally foraging whales [Fig. 4(d)]. The surface, dive, and bottom durations were also much shorter than the corresponding natural durations in Figs. 4(a) 4(c) and 6(a) 6(c). The contrast in bottom duration time is particularly striking in that tagged whales displaying shallow depredation generally conducted longer natural dives, when compared with the natural bottom time distribution for deep depredating animals [Fig. 6(c), second box plot]. Finally, dive inflection rates were twice as high during shallow depredation than during natural situations. Given the strong visual contrasts in the dive parameter distributions in Figs. 4 and 6, it is not TABLE II. Acoustic statistics for 2007 and 2009 data. Values are means þ/ s.d. over the number of dives N d considered. A p-value is the probability that the distribution is drawn from the same distribution as the natural foraging distribution. Italic p-values indicate the rejection of the null-hypothesis of a common underlying distribution (p < 0.005). T Cl1 (m) Cl (s 1 ) ICI (s) Cr þ CrP (h 1 ) F CrP (%) Resting N/A N/A N/A N/A N/A Natural foraging 0.9 þ/ 0.3 0.7 þ/ 0.2 0.8 þ/ 0.2 7.9 þ/ 4.6 48.4 þ/ 10.2 Deep depredation 0.5 þ/ 0.4 0.8 þ/ 0.3 0.4 þ/ 0.1 11.3 þ/ 7.1 58.4 þ/ 13.0 p ¼ 0.03 p ¼ 0.08 p 0.005 p < 0.005 p ¼ 0.08 Shallow depredation 0.1 þ/ 0.1 1.9 þ/ 0.4 0.3 þ/ 0.1 34.5 þ/ 15.2 68.3 þ/ 16.4 p < 0.005 p 0.005 p 0.005 p 0.005 p ¼ 0.04 526 J. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 132, No. 1, July 2012 Mathias et al.: Acoustic behavior of depredating sperm whales

FIG. 6. (Color online) Boxplots of same dive parameters shown in Fig. 4, but only using tag records that display both natural and depredation behavior. For each parameter, distributions are shown for deep and shallow depredation states, along with the distributions for the natural foraging states preceding/following a given depredation behavior. The box plots show the 5th, 25th, 50th, 75th, and 95th percentile values of the distributions. surprising that the KS tests yield highly significant p-values for all dive parameters (Tables I and III). 2. Dive parameters during deep depredation The seven other tagged whales seen surfacing consistently next to a fishing vessel were assigned to the deep depredation category. The differences between deep depredation and natural diving behavior were subtler than for shallow depredation. Deep-depredating whales had shorter surface, dive, and bottom durations when compared to the complete distribution of natural foraging samples [Figs. 4(a) 4(c)]; however, when the natural foraging dive samples were restricted to tag records displaying deep depredation only, the differences became much smaller, except for the surface duration [Fig. 6(a)]. The KS tests of the distributions in Fig. 6 (Tables I and III) still showed statistically significant differences for surface and dive durations. At first glance Fig. 4(d) suggests that depredation dives were slightly deeper than natural foraging dives; for example, 40% of deep-depredation dives exceeded 600 m vs only 15% of all natural foraging dives. But when the natural dive samples were restricted to those conducted by deep depredating whales only [Fig. 6(d)], one found little visible difference between the maximum dive depths, and the p-value for that analysis was not significant (p ¼ 0.04). The dive inflection rate distribution varied little between natural foraging and deep depredation and was not statistically significant. Interestingly, two of the ten depredating whales also performed either one or two resting dives, just before or after FIG. 7. (Color online) Boxplots of same acoustic parameters shown in Fig. 5, but only using tag records that display both natural and depredation behavior. For each parameter, distributions are shown for deep and shallow depredation states, along with the distributions for the natural foraging states preceding/following a given depredation behavior. The box plots show the 5th, 25th, 50th, 75th, and 95th percentile values of the distributions. J. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 132, No. 1, July 2012 Mathias et al.: Acoustic behavior of depredating sperm whales 527

TABLE III. Dive statistics for 2007 and 2009 data, comparisons within same tag record only. Values are means þ/ s.d. over the number of dives N d considered. N tag corresponds to the number of tag records displaying each behavior. N ind corresponds to the number of individuals displaying each behavior. A p-value is the probability that the distribution is drawn from the same distribution as the natural foraging distribution. Italic p-values indicate the rejection of the null-hypothesis of a common underlying distribution (p < 0.005). D max (m) T s (min) T d (min) T b (min) Infl (h 1 ) N d N tag N ind Resting 31.7 þ/ 3.5 6.1 þ/ 0.6 15.9 þ/ 2.3 7.1 þ/ 1.6 5.0 þ/ 1.8 10 3 3 Natural foraging associated with deep depredation 481.6 þ/ 152.7 9.2 þ/ 2.5 30.0 þ/ 7.7 12.6 þ/ 5.0 15.6 þ/ 7.0 66 4 2 Natural foraging associated with shallow depredation 417.4 þ/ 119.3 8.9 þ/ 2.0 35.0 þ/ 8.1 16.7 þ/ 8.3 14.8 þ/ 9.3 64 3 3 Deep depredation 487.2 þ/ 160.4 7.4 þ/ 2.4 28.4 þ/ 9.4 11.7 þ/ 6.2 19.1 þ/ 10.8 29 4 2 p ¼ 0.04 p < 0.005 p < 0.005 p ¼ 0.04 p ¼ 0.05 Shallow depredation 145.1 þ / 108.2 3.9 þ/ 2.6 13.8 þ/ 8.9 7.1 þ/ 5.0 35.1 þ/ 16.9 35 3 3 p 0.005 p 0.005 p 0.005 p 0.005 p 0.005 a series of shallow or deep depredating dives. Those resting dives thus happened in the middle of the day, while resting dives observed during natural foraging behavior usually happened during the night. This suggests that depredating dives might be more energy consuming than normal foraging dives, that depredating animals become satiated and can rest before resuming natural feeding, and/or that whales might rest while waiting for the start of a haul. 3. Acoustic parameters during shallow depredation Shallow depredating animals were very active acoustically, when compared with natural foraging behavior (Figs. 5 and 7, Tables II and IV). They continuously produced usual clicks, even when resting at the surface, and they began clicking almost immediately after starting a dive [Figs. 5(a) and 7(a)]. Their median click rate was nearly double over that displayed by naturally foraging dives; the reason for this difference is that the inter-click interval during shallow depredation was much smaller; over 75% of clicks during this behavior had an ICI less than 0.4, an interval never observed during natural behavior. Similar patterns arose for creak production rates; the median creak rate of 30 creaks/h during shallow depredation was more than three times higher than natural creak rates. The spread in creak rate values was also very high during shallow depredation: the 25th and 75th percentiles lie between 10 and 50 creaks per hour, respectively, while the spread was much narrower for natural dives. There was much less dramatic change in the median percentage of creak-pause events during shallow depredation; however, Fig. 5(e) shows that 75% of dives had creak-pause percentages exceeding 58%, while only 40% of natural foraging dives exceeded this fraction. Figure 7(e) shows that this difference persisted even when the natural foraging dives were restricted to individuals that also display shallow depredation. However, the large spread in the creak-pause percentage distributions under both behavioral conditions resulted in a nonsignificant p-value from the KS test (0.04), the only acoustic parameter for shallow depredation not to differ significantly from natural behavior. Shallow depredation dives showed another interesting acoustic effect, in that both click and the creak rates are generally lower during natural foraging dives that occurred after a haul is completed (e.g., Fig. 3). One possible interpretation of this result is that whales become satiated or tired after shallow depredation. 4. Acoustic parameters during deep depredation The acoustic behavior of deep depredating whales showed significant differences from natural foraging behavior as well. One noticeable behavioral shift was that deep depredating whales displayed significantly shorter ICIs (median 0.5 s) vs a median value of 0.75 s for the same individuals during natural dives [Fig. 7(c)] and a median value of 0.85 s for all natural foraging dives from all individuals. The KS test confirmed that a shift in the ICI distribution was the most significant effect (lowest p-value). Surprisingly, the average click rate of deep depredating whales did not change significantly, despite the noticeable shift in ICI levels. In TABLE IV. Acoustic statistics for 2007 and 2009 data, comparisons within same tag record. Values are means þ/ s.d. over the number of dives N d considered. A p-value is the probability that the distribution is drawn from the same distribution as the natural foraging distribution. Italic p-values indicate the rejection of the null-hypothesis of a common underlying distribution (p < 0.005). T Cl1 (m) Cl (s 1 ) ICI (s) Cr þ CrP (h 1 ) F CrP (%) Resting N/A N/A N/A N/A N/A Natural foraging associated with deep depredation 0.6 þ/ 0.2 0.8 þ/ 0.2 0.6 þ/ 0.2 8.7 þ/ 4.9 53.9 þ/ 9.3 Natural foraging associated with shallow depredation 1.2 þ/ 0.3 0.7 þ/ 0.3 0.8 þ/ 0.1 6.3 þ/ 3.5 46.7 þ/ 7.0 Deep depredation 0.5 þ/ 0.4 0.8 þ/ 0.3 0.4 þ/ 0.1 11.3 þ/ 7.1 58.4 þ/ 13.0 p < 0.005 p ¼ 0.2 p 0.005 p 0.005 p ¼ 0.07 Shallow depredation 0.1 þ/ 0.1 1.9 þ/ 0.4 0.3 þ/ 0.1 34.5 þ/ 15.2 68.3 þ/ 16.4 p < 0.005 p 0.005 p 0.005 p 0.005 p ¼ 0.04 528 J. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 132, No. 1, July 2012 Mathias et al.: Acoustic behavior of depredating sperm whales