Schooling Behavior of Thayeria. obliqua and Pristella maxillaris

Similar documents
ONSET OF SHOALING PREFERENCE IN PUNTIUS SARANA SUBNASUTUS

Key Words: Attraction, Color Cue, and Wavelength. Introduction

Conditioned Alarm Behavior in Fathead Minnows (Pimephales promelas) and Test Their Ability

Workshop on Predation Thomas Herbert, Ph.D. (revised by Dana Krempels in 2013)

Mixed-species shoaling in fish: the sensory mechanisms and costs of shoal choice

Dangerously bold Featured scientist: Melissa Kjelvik from Michigan State University

SCHOOLING BEHAVIOR OF HAEMULON SPP. IN BERMUDA REEFS AND SEAGRASS BEDS

EFFECTS OF PREDATION ON THE BEHAVIOR OF GAMMARUS MINUS

Dangerously bold Featured scientist: Melissa Kjelvik from Michigan State University

Tracking Juvenile Summer Flounder

The Life History of Triturus v. vittatus (Urodela) in Various Habitats

Key words: community similarity; coral patch reef; Enewetak; reeffish; species diversity; Virgin Islands.

Digital ESF. SUNY College of Environmental Science and Forestry. Devon Shaw. Joe Rubenstein. Shane Currey. Andrew Koch

A few things to remember about ANOVA

Preliminary results of SEPODYM application to albacore. in the Pacific Ocean. Patrick Lehodey

SPECIES RICHNESS IN THREE OCEANFLOOR HABITATS IN BERMUDA BAYS

Caddisfly Larvae Visual System: Response to Light

The Impact of Wolf Reintroduction on the Foraging Efficency of Elk and Bison

Craig P. Seltenrich Pacific Gas & Electric Company 3400 Crow Canyon Road San Ramon, California Introduction

Comparative growth of pinfish in field mesocosms across marsh, oyster, and soft-bottom habitat types in a Mississippi estuary

Video-Based Mapping of Oyster Bottom in the Upper Piscataqua River, Sturgeon Creek, and Spruce Creek

STUDY PERFORMANCE REPORT

EFFECTS OF PREDATION ON FOUNTAIN DARTERS STUDY

Predator-Prey Interactions: Bean Simulation. Materials

Impact on Base Population Density and Hunter Performance of Stocking with Pen-Raised Bobwhite

MRI-2: Integrated Simulation and Safety

Abstract. Introduction. Keywords: oyster, history, management, restoration, recruitment

IMPROVING POPULATION MANAGEMENT AND HARVEST QUOTAS OF MOOSE IN RUSSIA

Factors Affecting Phonotaxis in Male House Crickets, Acheta domesticus

CHAP Summary 8 TER 155

Tennessee Black Bear Public Opinion Survey

Factors influencing production

Report to the Benjamin Hair-Just Swim For Life Foundation on JACS4 The Jefferson Area Community Survey

DOI /HORIZONS.B P23 UDC : (497.11) PEDESTRIAN CROSSING BEHAVIOUR AT UNSIGNALIZED CROSSINGS 1

It s Cold Outside: Exploring the Effects of Temperature on GloFish Activity

Zooplankton Availability to. Larval Walleye (Sander vitreus) in Black Lake, MI, USA

THE DIET OF Lutra canadensis IN THE UPPER COLORADO RIVER SYSTEM

Evaluation of pedestrians speed with investigation of un-marked crossing

Exhibit Show (Use handout in Visitor s Guide to determine show times) (6 pts): 1. What was the name of the presentation that you attended?

Growth of juvenile black sea bass, Centropristis striata, fed either a commercial salmon or trout diet

SEDAR31-DW21. Examining delayed mortality in barotrauma afflicted red snapper using acoustic telemetry and hyperbaric experimentation

INFLUENCE OF ENVIRONMENTAL PARAMETERS ON FISHERY

2) Jensen, R. Comparison of ground-reaction forces while kicking a stationary and non-stationary soccer ball

Fashion a Michigan Fish

Laboratory 2 Locomotion, gravity responses and trail following in a terrestrial snail

BEFORE YOU OPEN ANY FILES:

POLYCULTURE OF LARGEMOUTH BASS (Micropterus salmoides) WITH BLUE TILAPIA (Oreochromis aurea): USING TILAPIA PROGENY AS FORAGE

Hatcheries: Role in Restoration and Enhancement of Salmon Populations

WARREN K. YUNKER, DAN E. WEIN, and BRIAN D. WISENDEN* Department of Biological Sciences University of Alberta Edmonton, Alberta, Canada T6G 2E9

Coral Bleaching and Climate Change Featured scientist: Carly Kenkel from The University of Texas at Austin

Remote Monitoring of Dolphins and Whales in the High Naval Activity Areas in Hawaiian Waters

Abstract. Introduction Fiber neps generation in cotton processing

Chapter 2: ANOVA and regression. Caroline Verhoeven

Oregon Hatchery Research Center January 2014 David L. G. Noakes, Professor & Director

STUDY PERFORMANCE REPORT

Factors Affecting Popularity of Zoo Animals. Including Diet, Exoticness, Sociality and Body Size

The Stickleback Fish - A Story of Modern Evolution

Fifty years ago, a single cod was large enough to feed a family of four or five. Today it is barely enough for one

Time Will Tell: Long-term Observations of the Response of Rocky-Habitat Fishes to Marine Reserves in Puget Sound

Final report on fish diet and stomach analyses

Using Actual Betting Percentages to Analyze Sportsbook Behavior: The Canadian and Arena Football Leagues

Camouflage for Life: Measuring the Adaptive Value of Color

Ecological Archives M A2

Improve pipetting results in pharmaceutical formulation by using MICROMAN E

Christina J. Slager Director of Animal Care and Exhibits. Aquarium of the Bay Pier 39, Embarcadero at Beach St. San Francisco, CA

Tide Gate Modifications for Fish Passage and Water Quality Enhancement

Catch per unit effort of coastal prawn trammel net fishery in Izmir Bay, Aegean Sea

tfishing A study of gender and life stage along New York s eastern Lake Ontario coast

Warning call of marmots

APPROACH RUN VELOCITIES OF FEMALE POLE VAULTERS

SEA GRANT PROGRESS REPORT

Fish Eating Contest List and explain the meaning of morphology, anatomy, and physiology (LA , 2, 3, 4, 5; SC.912.L.14.7).

Attachment 2 PETITIONERS

A comprehensive evaluation of the methods for evolving a cooperative team

Zooplankton Migration Patterns at Scotton Landing: Behavioral Adaptations written by Lauren Zodl, University of Delaware

Southern Oregon Coastal Cutthroat Trout

Grand Canyon Near Shore Ecology Study

Protect Our Reefs Grant Interim Report (October 1, 2008 March 31, 2009) Principal investigators: Donald C. Behringer and Mark J.

Tip Seal Behavior in Scroll Compressors

Killingly Public Schools

Cambridge International Examinations Cambridge International Advanced Subsidiary and Advanced Level

Proposed Pensacola Fish Hatchery: Potential Biological Impacts and Best Practices. Will Patterson

Analysis of Variance. Copyright 2014 Pearson Education, Inc.

FY 2015 ANNUAL PROJECT REPORT PROJECT NUMBER: 163

SCIENTIFIC COMMITTEE SEVENTH REGULAR SESSION August 2011 Pohnpei, Federated States of Micronesia

Effects on Auditory Attention and Walking While Texting with a Smartphone and Walking on Stairs

Sea urchin density along a depth gradient at Rodney's Rock and Champagne Bay, Dominica

OXYGEN CONSUMPTION AND TEMPERATURE IN THE AQUATIC ENVIRONMENT

Juvenile Salmon Use of Knik Arm Estuaries. Hannah N. Ramage

2009 New Brunswick Gambling Prevalence Study

Shoaling decisions in female swordtails: how do fish gauge group size?

ALL-NEW ACCESS EXPLORER S GUIDE. Rescue Care Conservation Research #parktoplanet

Vulnerability in small-scale coastal fisheries from northern coast of Sao Paulo, Brazil, in face of global environmental change

Build Your Own Zooplankton

How to Hide in the Ocean

Productivity per unit area (m 2 ) Total Productivity (global)

Fighting for Life in French Creek

Some Biological Parameters of Bigeye and Yellowfin Tunas Distributed in Surrounding Waters of Taiwan

Where Do We Come From? An Introduction to Primate Biology GK-12 Inquiry Science Lesson Kristin De Lucia Fall 2002

Abundance of Steelhead and Coho Salmon in the Lagunitas Creek Drainage, Marin County, California

Transcription:

Schooling Behavior of Thayeria obliqua and Pristella maxillaris Nicole Shrestha Biology 493 26 April 2006

Abstract This project tested schooling preferences of two species, Thayeria obliqua and Pristella maxillaris. The study used two 15-gallon aquariums, two screw-top jars, and twelve individuals of each species. Two vertical lines were drawn on the front of one aquarium, separating it into three equal sections. Fish were placed in jars at opposite ends of the aquarium, a test fish was released in the center of the tank, and movements were recorded over a fifteen minute period. Each test was replicated five times with individuals of each species. Only T. obliqua showed a significant preference for schooling with conspecific groups. Neither species showed a significant preference for the size of the group. Thayeria obliqua may occur in open water habitat where schooling would be adaptive. Pristella maxillaris lives in swampy, vegetative habitats where cover from predators does not require schooling. Introduction Research has shown that fish schools increase predator avoidance and foraging efficiency (Griffiths and Magurran 1997). A fish school is classified as a group in which all individuals swim together at the same speed (Wilson and Wilson 1985), and usually includes individuals of the same size and species (Marshall 1966). Fish schools afford protection from predators and maximize the individual s fitness (Barber and Ruxton 2000). Individuals prefer to join groups of familiar conspecifics instead of groups with which they have no experience (Barber and Ruxton 2000). The probability of mixedspecies schooling increases the closer the phylogenetic relationship of the two species (Ward et al. 2003). Barber and Ruxton (2000) demonstrated that sticklebacks kept jointly in schools for a six week period maintained cohesive schools when later permitted to freely interact with other schools. Ward et al. (2003) found that juvenile chub (Leuciscus cephalus and Phoxinus phoxinus) assembled preferentially with familiar fishes, regardless of whether or not they were conspecifcs. When presented with two nonfamiliar schools, the preference for conspecifics over heterospecifics disappeared when - 1 -

the heterospecifc school was composed of individuals familiar to the test fish (Ward et al. 2003). Krause et al. (1998) noted that group size influences foraging success and protection against predation. The evolutionary costs and benefits of schooling may depend on the size of the fish school (Krause et al. 1998). A comparison of school size choice by Threespine Stickleback (Gasterostreus aculeatus) and the Creek Chub (Semolitus atromaculatus), revealed that because of their spines and bony lateral plates, sticklebacks were less inclined to actively select foraging groups (Krause et al. 1998). Creek Chub were more vulnerable and responded to slight school-size differences, quickly choosing the larger of two schools. Species that are more susceptible to predation responded to differences in the size of conspecific schools (Krause et al. 1998). The purposes of this study was to test whether or not a single test fish preferred to school with heterospecifics or conspecifcs and to test how the number of fish in the school affected these decisions. Methods and Materials The methods followed those developed by Keenleyside (1968). Two 15-gallon aquariums and twelve individuals each of Thayeria obliqua (Penguin Tetra) and Pristella maxillaris (Gold Tetra) were used in the experiment. On the front of each aquarium, two vertical lines were drawn, separating it into three equal sections. To test for heterospecificity versus conspecificity, six to eight fish of each species were placed into separate screw-top jars (~ 4L). A jar of each species was placed at each end of the aquarium. A single test fish of one of the two species was carefully released in the center of the aquarium. Over a fifteen minute period, the time the test fish spent in each of the - 2 -

three areas of the tank was recorded. The test fish was then removed, and the position of the jars was reversed to test for any left/right tendencies. The same test fish was then returned to the aquarium and the recording procedure was repeated. Five replicates of this procedure were conducted with five different test fish of each species. To test for school size preference in P. maxillaris, the same aquarium and a similar set up was used. Two jars, one containing four conspecifics and one with only two conspecifics, were placed in opposite ends of the aquarium. An individual P. maxillaris was released in the center of the aquarium. The time this fish spent in each of the three areas was recorded over a fifteen minute period. The test fish was then removed, the positions of the jars reversed, and the test fish returned to the tank to test for any left/right tendencies. Five replicates of this test were performed. For the tests involving T. obliqua, five conspecifics were placed in one jar and three in the other. Five replicates were performed with this species following the same protocol used on P. maxillaris. Data were analyzed using analysis of variance (ANOVA) and Tukey post hoc tests. Results During a 15 minute test for preference between conspecific or heterospecific groups, P. maxillaris spent an average of 310 seconds ( ± 250 SD) in the area with conspecifics. In the area of the tank without fish, P. maxillaris spent an average of 338 seconds (± 263 SD), and in the area with heterospecifics they spent an average of 213 seconds (± 285 SD.)(Fig. 1). - 3 -

700 600 500 400 300 Time (sec) 200 100 0-100 -200 Conspecifics No Fish Zone Heterospecifics Figure 1 - The average time spent in each area of the tank by Pristella maxillaris when tested for species preference, including standard error bars. Differences between observations for each group were then analyzed for significance using an ANOVA test, with a resulting p-value >0.05, which indicated no significance. Thayeria obliqua was also tested for preference between conspecific and heterospecfic groups. They spent an average of 755 seconds (± 190 SD) out of the 15 minute test period in the area with conspecifics. In the area without fish they spent an average of 85 seconds (± 119 SD) and in the area with heterospecifics they spent an average of 17 seconds (± 22 SD)(Fig. 2). These differences were significant (p-value <0.05). A Tukey post hoc test showed a significant difference (p < 0.05) between the mean of time spent in the area with conspecifics and the means of the other two areas (Fig. 3). - 4 -

1000 800 600 Time (sec) 400 200 0-200 Conspecifics No Fish Zone Heterospecifics Figure 2 - The average time spent in each area of the tank by Thayeria obliqua when tested for species preference, with standard error bars shown. Figure 3 - The results from a Tukey test performed with the data from Thayeria obliqua's tests for preference between conspecfics and heterospecifics. Mean 1 represents the area of the tank with conspecifics, mean 2 represents the area of the tank without fish, and mean 3 represents the area of the tank with heterospecifics. Cell No. Var1 {1} 755.17 {2} 84.833 {3} 16.833 1 1-0.000178 0.000178 2 2 0.000178-0.645700 3 3 0.000178 0.645700 - When tested for group size preference, P. maxillaris spent an average of 345 seconds (± 164 SD) out of a 15 minute period in the area with four conspecifics. In the no fish zone, they spent an average of 253 seconds (± 94 SD). In the area with two conspecifics, they spent an average of 233 seconds (± 249 SD) (Fig. 3). - 5 -

600 500 400 Time (sec) 300 200 100 0-100 4 Fish No Fish Zone 2 Fish Figure 3 - Pristella maxillaris's average time spent in each area of the tank when tested for group size preference, including standard error bars. These data were not significant (p <0.05). Thayeria obliqua was also tested for group size preference and spent an average of 420 seconds (± 357 SD) in the area with five conspecifics. In the no fish zone, they spent an average of 135 seconds (± 104 SD). In the area with three conspecifics, they spent an average of 319 seconds (± 347 SD)(Fig. 4). These data were not significantly different from each other (p >0.05). 900 800 700 600 500 Time (sec) 400 300 200 100 0-100 5 Fish No Fish Zone 3 Fish Figure 4 - The average time spent in each area of the tank by Thayeria obliqua when tested for group size preference, with standard error bars. - 6 -

Discussion and Conclusions When the two species were tested for preference between conspecifics and heterospecifics, only T. obliqua demonstrated a significant preference for conspecifics. Pristella maxillaris showed no preference for any area of the tank. The difference between the two species preferences for certain areas of the tank was most likely due to the habitats where the fish naturally occur. Boujard et al. (1997) notes that P. maxillaris are commonly found in calm coastal waters and in swamps with dense vegetation throughout the Amazon, Orinoco, and Guianas river basins. It is possible that because P. maxillaris are found in areas with dense vegetation, they use the vegetation as a method of predator evasion. If this is true, there would be less selection pressure to form schools. Lima et al. (2003) note that T. obliqua are found throughout the middle Amazon, middle Tocantins, and Guaporé river basins. The literature does not reveal whether T. obliqua is an open water fish in these regions, but if it is, this would explain its inclination to school as do many other open water fish. When tested for group size preference, neither species showed a significant preference for any area of the tank. The results from the first experiment indicate that P. maxillaris do not have strong schooling tendencies, so it is understandable why there was no significant preference for group size. However, explaining T. obliqua s lack of preference is more difficult. One possible explanation is that as long as other fish are near, an individual may not be able to differentiate between group sizes. Another explanation could be that the difference between the two groups was not large enough to have an effect on an individual s behavior. - 7 -

Sources Cited Barber, I. and G.D. Ruxton. 2000. The importance of stable schooling: do familiar sticklebacks stick together? Proceedings: Biological Sciences 267 (1439): 151-155. Boujard, T., M. Pascal, J.F. Meunier and P.Y. Le Bail. 1997. Poissons de Guyane. Guide écologique de l'approuague et de la réserve des Nouragues. Institut National de la Recherche Agronomique. Paris. pp.219 Griffiths, S.W. and A. Magurran. 1997. Schooling preferences for familiar fish vary with group size in a wild guppy population. Proceedings: Biological Sciences 264 (1381): 547-551. Keenleyside, M.H.A. 1986. Chapter Eight In: Animal Behavior in Laboratory and Field. A.W. Stokes ed. pp. 35-38. W.H. Freeman and Company. San Francisco. Krause, J., J-G.J Godin, and D. Rubenstein. 1998. Group choice as a function of group size differences and assessment time in fish: the influence of species vulnerability to preadation. Ethology 104 (1): 68-74. Lima, F.C.T., L.R. Malabarba, P.A. Buckup, J.F. Pezzi da Silva, R.P. Vari, A. Harold, R. Benine, O.T. Oyakawa, C.S. Pavanelli, N.A. Menezes, C.A.S. Lucena, M.C.S.L. Malabarba, Z.M.S. Lucena, R.E. Reis, F. Langeani, L. Cassati and V.A. Bertaco. 2003. Genera Incertae Sedis in Characidae. In: Checklist of the Freshwater Fishes of South and Central America. R.E. Reis, S.O. Kullander and C.J. Ferraris, Jr. eds. pp.106-168. Edipucrs. Porto Alegre. Marshall, N.B. 1996. The Life of Fishes. Cleveland: World Publishing Company: pp. 402. Ward, A.J.W., S. Axford, and J. Krause. 2003. Cross-species familiarity in shoaling fishes. Proceedings: Biological Sciences 270 (1520) 1157-1161. Wilson, R. and J.Q. Wilson. 1985. Watching Fishes: Life and Behavior on Coral Reefs. New York: Harper and Row. 239 pp. - 8 -