The Basis of The Self-Explaining Road

Similar documents
HUMAN (DRIVER) ERRORS

Exemplary Conditional Automation (Level 3) Use Case Description Submitted by the Experts of OICA as input to the IWG ITS/AD

Road design and Safety philosophy, 1 st Draft

The key to achievement of a sustainable, safe traffic system lies in the systematic and consistent application of safety principles.

Emergency Rides. Driving Simulators Research Development Production. 1. General. Rev

DOI /HORIZONS.B P23 UDC : (497.11) PEDESTRIAN CROSSING BEHAVIOUR AT UNSIGNALIZED CROSSINGS 1

SUMMARY OF SAFETY INVESTIGATION REPORT


E4 Cycle Route Exeter University to Redhayes Bridge. - Recommendations from Exeter Cycling Campaign

Identify the letter of the choice that best completes the statement or answers the question.

THE PRINCIPLES OF A SUSTAINABLE STREET NETWORK AND SAFE TRAFFIC SYSTEM IN LITHUANIAN CITIES

This objective implies that all population groups should find walking appealing, and that it is made easier for them to walk more on a daily basis.

Introduction Definition of decision-making: the capacity of the player to execute an action following some conscious tactical or strategical choice.

A method for analysing the traffic process in a safety perspective

CITY OF SAINT JOHN TRAFFIC CALMING POLICY

Operation and safety of tramways in interaction with public space. COST Action TU1103 «STATE OF THE ART» On behalf of the action: Matus Sucha

TRIM Queue, Vejle N Denmark. Evaluation report

A Traffic Operations Method for Assessing Automobile and Bicycle Shared Roadways

Town of Babylon Sustainable Complete Streets Policy

Project MAVEN. Preliminary survey results. Ondřej Přibyl Czech Technical University in Prague Czech Republic. October 2018, Greenwich

Coquitlam Cross-town Bike Route Improving Bicycle Facilities in a Mature Suburban Environment

Soft measures affect traffic in Lund effects from two years work with a sustainable transport system in Lund

A review of traffic safety in Finnish municipalities

TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF FIGURES. Figure Title

Analyses and statistics on the frequency and the incidence of traffic accidents within Dolj County

At-Grade Intersections versus Grade-Separated Interchanges (An Economic Analysis of Several Bypasses)

Unit Six: Driving Faster with More Risk URBAN, SUBURBAN, AND RURAL DRIVING

Learning event following Ironmen runaway

The automobile is a complex system in which humans play an important role. Driving is

Trial 3: Interactions Between Autonomous Vehicles and Pedestrians and Cyclists

Children s risks on their way to school: the example of Tallinn

LECTUR 10 CHARACTERISTICS OF THE DRIVER, THE PEDESTRIAN, THE VEHICLE AND THE ROAD. One problem that faces traffic and transportation engineers is:

VRU Safety & Traffic Calming Measures in Urban & Rural Areas

10.0 CURB EXTENSIONS GUIDELINE

Understanding school road safety

SAFE DESIGN OF RURAL ROADS BY NORMALIZED ROAD CHARACTERISTICS

2.0 LANE WIDTHS GUIDELINE

ROSEE RSA/RSI Introduction courses ROSEE Speed Management courses

Appendix A. Knoxville TPO Greenway Signage Guidelines. Appendix A: Knoxville TPO Greenway Signage Guidelines Knox to Oak Ridge Greenway Master Plan

Pedestrian Crosswalk Audit

Living Streets Policy

At each type of conflict location, the risk is affected by certain parameters:

EVERYONE IS A PEDESTRIAN CROSSING ADVICE FOR PEDESTRIANS

Coast Riders Motorcycle Club. Group Ride Guidelines

ROAD TRAFFIC SAFETY IN BELARUS CURRENT SITUATION

Road Safety Assessment

Cyclists gaze behavior in urban space: an eye-tracking experiment on bicycle facilities.

Task 4 Wayfinding Elements, Placement and Technical Guidance 4.1 Wayfinding Elements

Cyclist Safety at Signalised Junctions

What s Up in Navigation Displays?

The Effect of Pavement Marking on Speed. Reduction in Exclusive Motorcycle Lane. in Malaysia

From road shares to road sharing: Cyclist-motorists interactions and its effect on cyclists' perceptions and willingness to share the road

Beginning School Bus Driver Curriculum UNIT IX. FIELD TRIPS and TRAFFIC CONTROL DEVICES. Revised 09/2011 Revised 10/2013 (Instructional Content)

Pedestrian Dynamics: Models of Pedestrian Behaviour

Why Zone In on Speed Reduction?

Introduction to Transportation Engineering. Discussion of Stopping and Passing Distances

4 Goals, Objectives & Actions

Planning and Design of Proposed ByPass Road connecting Kalawad Road to Gondal Road, Rajkot - Using Autodesk Civil 3D Software.

Truck accidents. Rikke Rysgaard Danish Road Traffic Accident Investigation Board

Pathways to a Healthy Decatur

Reduction of Speed Limit at Approaches to Railway Level Crossings in WA. Main Roads WA. Presenter - Brian Kidd

FLIGHT CREW TRAINING NOTICE

A CHANCE TO RIDE. Lesson 3 Pedestrian Safety & Awareness of Surroundings

The role of street traffic signs in reducing road accidents

Findings on the Effectiveness of Intersection Treatments included in the Victorian Statewide Accident Black Spot Program

summary of issues and opportunities

Road Users Safety Effective Measures. Alain Areal

UNIT V 1. What are the traffic management measures? [N/D-13] 2. What is Transportation System Management (TSM)? [N/D-14]

Do LED-advertising signs affect driver attention?

Simulation Analysis of Intersection Treatments for Cycle Tracks

CITY OF WEST KELOWNA COUNCIL POLICY MANUAL

MICROSIMULATION USING FOR CAPACITY ANALYSIS OF ROUNDABOUTS IN REAL CONDITIONS

Texas Driver Education Classroom and In-car Instruction Model Curriculum

Economic and Social Council

Grip on speed. A multi-stakeholder approach to road safety

A GUIDE TO GROUP RIDING FOR BEGINNERS By Colin Hughes, Phoenix CC (Cycling Ulster)

TomTom South African Congestion Index

[Bicycle] Bells and Whistles to Improve Your Audits and Research. Jonny Rotheram. Rail~Volution - October LA 1

Pedestrian and Driver Distraction: Overview & NHTSA Prevalence and Risk Study. Heidi Coleman, NHTSA Bob Scopatz, VHB

Post impact trajectory of vehicles at rural intersections

Harley Owners Group. Chapter #1590. Chapter Member Orientation Guide. and. Group Riding Information

Low Level Cycle Signals with an early release Appendices

Introduction to Match Officiating (L1)

Crash Patterns in Western Australia. Kidd B., Main Roads Western Australia Willett P., Traffic Research Services

ENCOURAGING TAXI DRIVERS TO BEHAVE: GRAFTON BRIDGE TAXI AND BUS LANE TRIAL

EXPERIMENTAL RESEARCH REGARDING NIGHT VISIBILITY IN ROAD TRAFFIC

GD 0043/18 ACTIVE TRAVEL STRATEGY

Unit 5: Prioritize and Manage Hazards and Risks STUDENT GUIDE

Bike Lanes at Intersections with Right Turn-Only Lanes

Basic organization of the training field

An Analysis of Reducing Pedestrian-Walking-Speed Impacts on Intersection Traffic MOEs

Bicycle/Bus Conflict Area Study

Advanced driver assistance systems status and future potential

CAVING REGISTRATION LEVELS AND ASSESSMENT REQUIREMENTS

Cycling and risk. Cycle facilities and risk management

Keywords: Highway Intersection, Intersection Accidents, Crash Type, Crash Contributing, Statistical Analysis, Design Factors

A GUIDE TO RISK ASSESSMENT IN SHIP OPERATIONS

Safety Effects of Converting Intersections to Roundabouts

Highway 406 Roundabout Positive Guidance Analysis

PLACEMENT OF SIGNS RECOMMENDED PRACTICES SUB-SECTION

Transcription:

1 Rural Road Design Meeting, Copenhagen April 2014 12-03-2014 Paper authored by Lene Herrstedt, M.Sc., Ph.D. lh@trafitec.dk The Basis of The Self-Explaining Road The concept of the self-explaining road is used in various contexts associated with demands and objectives to improve traffic conditions and enhancing safety. But exactly what does that mean, and how do we create self-explaining roads? Explanatory model for road user behaviour A new report (Ref. 1) describes how we as drivers behave in the traffic and how our behaviour is influenced by road design and traffic environment. The description presents an explanatory model for road user behaviour, the purpose being to establish a practical tool of analysis to be used for the understanding of causes of various problems on the road network. The best basis for finding the solution is to understand the problem. The conclusions of this model form the basis of the selfexplanatory road. Nordic human factors guideline The explanatory model was developed as part of the project Nordic Human Factors Guideline. This is a Nordic collaborative project under the Nordic Road Geometry Group. In brief, the project covers road users demands to the design and construction of road systems based on existing knowledge about the physical and mental abilities of road users. The aim of the project has been to collect a fairly comprehensive knowledge on road user behaviour and disseminate such knowledge in a short and manageable form to technicians and planners working in the practical field of improving traffic conditions and road safety. The work has been carried out by a Nordic research team represented by Lene Herrstedt (Civil Engineer, MSc., Ph.D., Trafitec, Denmark), Gabriel Helmers (Traffic Psychologist, Ph.D., Consultant, Sweden) and Fridulv Sagberg (Traffic Psychologist, Senior Researcher, TOI, Norway). The project includes four sub-projects (see Figure 1). This article deals with Project II Development of an explanatory model for road user behaviour

2 Development of an explanatory model for road user behaviour In order to design good road and street environments, it is necessary to get a thorough understanding of how we function as road users. Based on existing knowledge about physical and mental abilities of road users, the explanatory model defines some general principles for road user behaviour. These basic characteristics of human behaviour are rooted in general scientific theories. Collecting and summarizing knowledge in more general terms is a necessary step in all theory and knowledge development. A good explanatory model is a prerequisite for better understanding of road user behaviour. The development of a practically usable explanatory model is a several-step process. Firstly, some of the basic characteristics of human behaviour are described. This leads to the formulation of general principles for road user behaviour. These general principles are then used as a tool to analyse and illustrate some very specific problem areas. This is done by using the model to attempt to predict behavioural incidents in traffic. Subsequently, the predictions should be either confirmed or rejected based on empirical studies. If rejected, the model will have to be adjusted. As such, the applicability of the model is tested several times, thus making the model operational. For this purpose, three problem areas have been defined: Ghost driving Speed choice and conditions for speed adaptation Drivers reading and understanding of road signs and markings Finally, the practical use of the newly developed explanatory model has been tested. In Part III of the project (see Figure 1), analyses were performed on a number of problem examples with the purpose of demonstrating the usability of the explanatory model as a tool for problem analysis and solving of actual traffic problems in practice. The selection of problems is made by Swedish, Danish, and Norwegian road specialists, working with the daily operation and improvement of traffic conditions. Road user behaviour in an evolutionally perspective Those species and individuals, who with the least possible effort have achieved the greatest possible benefit, have survived through the ages (Darwin s theory of evolution). Through this sorting principle man, with his brain, senses and movement apparatus, is built to function as an effective whole in the environment. When moving around in traffic we act rationally and effectively based on the principle greatest possible benefit with the least possible effort. We try to avoid detours by choosing the shortest/fastest possible route with no obstacles. Even though man is the most developed species on earth, we are born with virtually no skills. We must therefore, as a general rule, acquire everything through learning. We do however, possess certain instincts from the very beginning, i.e. we have an instinctive sense of fear of heights and a natural understanding of the fact that we could hurt ourselves if falling down. In contrast to this, we do not in the same immediate way perceive the speed limits we uphold when driving as particularly dangerous to us. This is something we have to learn.

3 A new-born child will have to learn what belongs to its own body (i.e. by biting a finger and experiencing the pain) and what does not (i.e. by biting a stick). By acting in our environment we constantly receive feedback on our behaviour and gradually we will lean to understand the features of our environment and its objects. Through such feedback we eventually learn how to achieve the benefits and avoid the dangers in our environment. This learning process repeats itself each time we need to acquire new skills. In a traffic related context, we must learn to walk, ride a bike, and drive a car. Once we have learned how to operate a vehicle, we consider this as an almost natural extension of our arms and legs. During the learning process, obviously there is a risk of making mistakes and get hurt. Only after several attempts and long experience we will obtain an adequately safe skill. This is confirmed by accident statistics for debut cyclists and drivers. We perceive the immediate surroundings as a whole Road user behaviour is based on his/her general holistic perception of the road and the immediate traffic situation. The information we require about our immediate surroundings is registered immediately by the brain through our senses. The brain automatically registers the contents of the visual information received through our eyes. In this way, without having to interpret the visual impressions, we form an immediate perception of the world around us. At the same time, our perception of the immediate situation is dynamic. This means that we register the direction we are moving in and what will happen shortly in the current space just before it actually happens. For instance, drivers often go in a direction straight towards the crossing pedestrian ahead because we see that the pedestrian will have already reached the sidewalk when we reach and pass the crossing. Our senses and brain are developed in interaction with the environment in a way that makes us receptive to the information we need. The more detailed the information we receive through our senses, the sooner we get a proper perception of the surroundings. In daylight it is much easier to get sufficiently detailed visual information compared to what we can achieve in the dark. This means that when driving in the dark, we may not always achieve an accurate perception of the road space. Worst case this can lead to misunderstanding of the traffic situation. Driving a car involves three main tasks Driving a car is a learned skill. For experienced drivers car driving is primarily an automated action which they perform without having to think more deeply about how they do it. A great deal of the time we can therefore think of other things while driving. Through his immediate holistic perception of the road environment and without even thinking about it the driver choses speed and position in such way that a comfortable safety margin can be maintained. The driver achieves this by driving in such way that his perceived safety margin in front of the car is longer than his perceived shortest stopping distance. The driver controls this by continuously adapting his driving speed. The perceived safety margin of the driver is subjective. At the same time a true (objective) but unknown safety margin exists. The driver loses control when he/she overestimates the objective

4 (true) safety margin. To ensure compliance between perceived and true safety margin, the driver must be sufficiently experienced. The driver solves tasks on three different mental levels: Control, Guidance, and Navigation. The control task is the most important one as this must be performed continuously during driving. It is performed automatically without conscious decisions whereby the driver automatically adjusts speed and position to maintain a comfortable perceived safety margin (see description above). As long as the driver needs to perform control tasks only, he has reserves of mental capacity to think of other things, talk to passengers or listen to the radio. The mental reserve capacity also means he has a wide range of awareness or a large functional field of vision enabling him to register what is happening on and along the road in a relatively long and broad sector in front of the car. The guidance task requires more capacity of the driver. When performing various manoeuvres such as lane changing or overtaking, the driver must make perceptual assessments in order to consciously decide whether a respective manoeuvre can be performed in the current traffic situation. Overcoming this task requires a high degree of focus and the driver s entire mental capacity. The functional field of vision is reduced and consequently the driver is at risk of overlooking traffic incidents which he/she would typically register during automated driving. The navigation task is the most complex and the only task out of the three which may be characterised as problem solving. As such, the task requires both understanding of symbols and logical thinking. Reading and understanding of road signs, guidance signs and road markings as well as planning a journey from A to B are navigation tasks. If the driver does not already know the way he will have to use the various kinds of road guidance signs whilst simultaneously orienting in the road system to determine Where am I? and Which direction should I choose? The choice of direction will be based on previous experiences and use of the symbolic information conveyed on road guidance signs, road maps and GPS. The navigation task requires the driver s full attention and takes up his entire mental capacity. Road signs will only be read when the driver has an immediate need for information The driver needs to - at least briefly - operate in navigation mode in order to perceive and utilise the information on a road sign or road guidance sign. If the driver continues in automated mode (control task) or performs a demanding manoeuvre (in guidance mode) he is likely to miss the road sign and hence the information. At automated driving the driver strives to maintain full control of the vehicle and he unconsciously registers from a long distance what is happening on the road ahead of the car. As soon as the driver observes a deviation from a distance, his curiosity and need for information are aroused. The driver will then slide into guidance mode enabling assessment of the situation. If he does not fully understand the situation, he begins functioning at navigation level by reading road signs etc. to collect information. When a driver is using a road for the first time he is curious about the appearance of the road. He will be attentive and careful. He will need time to identify possible dangerous passages and will therefore need timely information from road signs, i.e. a warning about a dangerous curve on the road. The warning sign must contribute to creating the proper expectations in the driver thus avoiding surprises.

5 However, when driving on a road he knows very well, the driver will know how to drive and which speed to pursue along each section of the road. In such case the driver will not be reading the road signs since he has no need for them, except for orientation in the dark. Good guidance Good guidance requires readable early warning signs before every major intersection indicating the driving direction to a limited number of well-known geographical destinations. Well-oriented in the environment The road network must be constructed so that road users will always know where they are located (landmarks), and so that orientation in this is easy (simplicity). The road user s expectations to the road and its continuation The experiences we as humans accumulate throughout life in terms of the surrounding world and how we adapt to the requirements we encounter are gathered in a bank of knowledge that we carry with us. We take advantage of this also when driving. Based on previous experience from similar roads and traffic environments we build up certain expectations about how to drive on the current road and which requirements it will pose on us. Even before entering a road for the first time, we have certain expectations as to how this looks. If the road is a national highway we expect the road standard to be relatively high. If the road is a minor road in a rural area, we expect it to be narrow and winding (Figure 3). We have a natural curiosity about how the road looks the very first time we enter it. But already after a few kilometres we have built up an expectation of how the continued road will look. The natural expectation is that the road will continue as it began. However, if the road worsens, we prepare ourselves to meet an even worse standard along the way. If the driver s perception and expectations to the road design and functionality do not correspond with the physical design and function, the driver will be surprised and confused. In a situation where prompt action is required, problems often occur because there is not enough time to take proper actions. Very often this will lead to human errors, which in turn can cause critical situations such as lack of reaction, excessively long reaction times, sudden and unexpected manoeuvres, etc. When a driver has taken the same road several times thereby obtaining a thorough knowledge about it, he has even stronger expectations for the appearance of the road when driving it the next time. If the road has been reconstructed since the driver s last visit, the design of the road may not correspond to the expectations. The driver will then immediately start to reorient himself in the road environment and base his knowledge on the immediate visual impression. The challenge for the driver is then, that he will need sufficient time to act in a new unexpected way, if required by the reconstructed road. Changes in the road environment pose increased demands on the driver. The road leading up to the change should therefore be constructed such that the driver will gradually lose recognition and thereby consciously begin to reorient in the new road environment.

6 When the road and the traffic conditions are in line with our expectations, no unexpected surprises occur. We then naturally have a high level of preparedness for the incidents and situations that we are expecting ahead. Correct expectations with respect to road design are therefore an important prerequisite for a well-adapted and safe driving. The Self-Explaining Road The general principles for road user behaviour defined in the Explanatory Model form the basis of the Self-Explaining Road: The Self-Explaining Road refers to a road which is designed in such way that road users immediately perceive how to drive on this Road user behaviour is mainly determined by the driver s expectations to and perception of the road and the current traffic situation. Consequently, the road must be designed to meet driver expectations. This means that the road must have a clear and recognisable standardised design. The Self-Explaining Road must make it easy for the road user to go right and difficult to go wrong. The idiom of the Self-Explaning Road should make symbolic information dispensable. Symbolic information (road signs and markings) shall primarily confirm the natural information expressed by the road design and must never be in conflict with this natural information. Today the self-explaning road is a long-term design goal for all road designs. Creating the selfexplaning road requires a continued development towards standardised, consistent and easily recognisable design characteristics for different types of roads and road sections of various functions. In this process it is very important that use of symbolic information always supplement and confirm the natural information of the road design without being in conflict. Recognition in the immediate holistic perception of the road and the traffic environment is a fundamental prerequisite to enable precise and accurate driver expectations of the road, both in terms of road design and traffic situations to prepare for. The Explanatory Model is a tool for use in the development process towards realizing the Self- Explaning Road. References Nordic Human Factors Guideline. Explanatory model for road user behavior. Trafitec (2014).

7 Nordic Human factors Guideline project includes four sub-projects: I) Collection of existing knowledge based on comprehensive literature studies is presented in five thematic notes on road users physical and mental abilities: Reaction time break response time and decision response time Vertical eye level, reading distance, and reading time for drivers Walking speeds Assessment of speed and distance Inattention and distraction II) III) IV) Development of an explanatory model for road user behavior with the purpose of increasing the understanding of how drivers act in traffic and how our actions are influenced by road design and traffic environment. Conclusions from the model form the basis of The self-explaining road Execution of a number of case analyses for verification of the applicability of the explanatory model as a tool for problem analysis and solution of specific traffic problems in practice. Three power point series with accompanying notes for the dissemination of the main findings of the project: The physical abilities of road users The mental abilities of road users Explanatory Model for road user behavior. All reports are available as downloads from www.nmfv.dk/vejgeometrigruppen Figure 1: The Nordic Human Factors Guideline Project includes four sub-projects and creates the basis for the self-explaining road. Figure 2: If the road is a national highway we expect the road standard to be relatively high.

Figure 3: If the road is a minor road in a rural area, we expect it to be narrow and winding 8