Locomotor sequence learning in visually guided walking

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Locomotor sequence learning in visually guided walking"

Transcription

1 J Neurophysiol 115: , First published February 10, 2016; doi: /jn Locomotor sequence learning in visually guided walking Julia T. Choi, 1,2 Peter Jensen, 2 and Jens Bo Nielsen 2 1 Department of Kinesiology, University of Massachusetts, Amherst, Massachusetts; and 2 Neural Control of Movement Research Group, Department of Neuroscience and Pharmacology and Department of Nutrition, Exercise and Sport, University of Copenhagen, Copenhagen, Denmark Submitted 8 October 2015; accepted in final form 3 February 2016 Choi JT, Jensen P, Nielsen JB. Locomotor sequence learning in visually guided walking. J Neurophysiol 115: , First published February 10, 2016; doi: /jn Voluntary limb modifications must be integrated with basic walking patterns during visually guided walking. In this study we tested whether voluntary gait modifications can become more automatic with practice. We challenged walking control by presenting visual stepping targets that instructed subjects to modify step length from one trial to the next. Our sequence learning paradigm is derived from the serial reaction-time (SRT) task that has been used in upper limb studies. Both random and ordered sequences of step lengths were used to measure sequence-specific and sequence-nonspecific learning during walking. In addition, we determined how age (i.e., healthy young adults vs. children) and biomechanical factors (i.e., walking speed) affected the rate and magnitude of locomotor sequence learning. The results showed that healthy young adults (age 24 5 yr, n 20) could learn a specific sequence of step lengths over 300 training steps. Younger children (age 6 10 yr, n 8) had lower baseline performance, but their magnitude and rate of sequence learning were the same compared with those of older children (11 16 yr, n 10) and healthy adults. In addition, learning capacity may be more limited at faster walking speeds. To our knowledge, this is the first study to demonstrate that spatial sequence learning can be integrated with a highly automatic task such as walking. These findings suggest that adults and children use implicit knowledge about the sequence to plan and execute leg movement during visually guided walking. human; learning; locomotion; vision; walking Address for reprint requests and other correspondence: J. Choi, Dept. of Kinesiology, Univ. of Massachusetts Amherst, 30 Eastman Lane, Amherst MA ( jtchoi@kin.umass.edu). THE AUTOMATIC CONTROL OF WALKING is critical to our daily activities. We normally do not consciously think about walking unless we encounter novel situations where we have to make voluntary gait modifications (e.g., hiking across stepping stones). On the other hand, voluntary control of stepping patterns can become more automatic with practice (e.g., line dancing). Locomotor skills can be learned via visual cues from the environment or proprioceptive cues from the limbs during obstacle avoidance tasks (Erni and Dietz 2001; Lajoie et al. 2012). Moreover, there is some evidence that visual cue training could improve gait patterns that transfer to walking without cues in people with Parkinson s disease (Spaulding et al. 2013). The classic serial reaction-time (SRT) task, first introduced by Nissen and Bullemer (1987), may be used to assess to what an extent a motor task has become automatic. The SRT task requires subjects to rapidly respond to different spatial cues by pressing corresponding buttons as fast and accurately as possible. With practice, subjects demonstrate learning by performing faster on the repeating sequence than on random sequences. This difference can only be accounted for by sequence-specific learning, which can occur with or without explicit knowledge (Cohen et al. 1990; Curran and Keele 1993; Nissen and Bullemer 1987). The primary objective of this study was to test whether spatial sequence learning could be integrated with a highly automatic task such as walking. We challenged walking control by presenting stepping targets of varying spatial complexities (e.g., random vs. ordered). To our knowledge, this is the first study to investigate sequence learning in the lower limbs during walking. Our second objective was to determine how age (i.e., healthy young adults vs. children) and biomechanical factors (i.e., walking speed) affect the rate and magnitude of locomotor sequence learning. Cortical control of gait is not fully developed in children under the age of 10 yr (Petersen et al. 2010), so we therefore predicted that visuomotor control of walking might be more difficult in younger children compared with adults. Furthermore, biomechanical constraints may supersede the goal of precision, in which case we would see a drop in performance as we challenge subjects to walk at faster speeds. METHODS Subjects. Twenty young adults (age 24 5 yr) and 18 children (mean age 11 3 yr, range 6 15 yr) with no known neurological disorders participated in this study. The study was approved by the local ethics committee (protocol #H-A ). All methods conformed to the Declaration of Helsinki. All subjects gave written informed consent prior to participation. Motion capture. Reflective markers were placed bilaterally on the toe (5th metatarsal head, 5MT), ankle (lateral malleolus), knee (lateral joint space), hip (greater trochanter), pelvis (iliac crest), and shoulder (acromion process) to capture walking kinematics (Fig. 1A). A sixcamera ProReflex motion capture system (Qualisys, Gothenburg, Sweden) was used to collect three-dimensional (3-D) marker data at 100 Hz. Identification of markers was performed using the AIM module in the Qualisys Track Manager software. Experimental setup and visual feedback. We created a visuomotor walking task where subjects must change their step length to hit visual targets while walking on a treadmill (Fig. 1A). Stepping targets and foot position were projected on a screen in front of the treadmill. The projector (Toshiba TDP-T 355) was mounted on the ceiling. The projection distance was 148 cm away from the screen, which resulted in a screen size of 125 cm high by 167 cm wide. This setup allowed us to manipulate the sequence of target locations step by step and to provide real-time visual feedback to the subject during the experiment. A custom-made computer program controlled the position of the targets while the QTM real-time server provided the position of the foot (i.e., 5MT marker) during the walking task. Targets were represented by cm open squares on the visual display (Fig. 1B). The swing leg s foot position was displayed /16 Copyright 2016 the American Physiological Society

2 LOCOMOTOR SEQUENCE LEARNING 2015 Downloaded from Fig. 1. A: visual feedback was projected on a screen in front of the treadmill. A 12-marker setup was used for motion capture. MT, metatarsal. B: target (open square) and foot position of the swing leg (filled circle) were displayed during the walking task. The targets moved down the screen at a speed corresponding to the treadmill speed (arrows). Stance leg foot position (open circle) was not visible on the display. The vertical distance between the current target (red square) and the next target (gray square) indicated the desired step length (1). The position of the swing leg appeared after ipsilateral toe off (2). The current target turned from red to white color on a successful hit (3). Scores were displayed on the upper right corner of the screen. The current target and foot position disappeared after ipsilateral heel strike (4). C: the order of random (R1 R3) and sequence blocks (S1 S4) in the sequence learning paradigm. Each block consisted of 100 steps. Sequence-specific learning was calculated as the difference in performance between S3 and R3 (solid line); nonspecific learning was calculated as the difference between R2 and R3 (dotted line). as an 8-cm-diameter circle. Stepping targets for the left foot and right foot appeared on the left and right of midline, respectively. The targets first appeared near the top of the screen and moved down at a speed corresponding to the treadmill speed (arrow). The subject saw both the current target (red square) and the next target (gray square). The vertical distance between the two targets indicated the required step length (Fig. 1B, 1). The position of the swing leg appears at ipsilateral toe off (Fig. 1B, 2). Subjects were instructed to step on the targets as accurately as possible. A successful hit was one where the center of the foot (circle) lay within 8 cm of the center of the target (square) after heel strike (Fig. 1B, 3). The current target turned white on each successful hit. The score was updated and displayed on the top right corner of the screen. The current target and foot position disappeared after ipsilateral heel strike (Fig. 1B, 4). The next target turned red and the foot position of the contralateral leg appeared. See Supplementary Video S1 for a replay of the visual display during the visuomotor task. (Supplemental material for this article is available online at the Journal of Neurophysiology website.) Learning paradigm. Each sequence was composed of three different step lengths (e.g., short, medium, long). The step lengths were adjusted proportionally to each subject s leg length. The medium step length was defined as two-thirds of leg length. The short step length was set at 80% of medium step length, and the long step length at 120% of medium step length. The average leg length was m in adults and m in children who participated in this study. Two speed conditions were tested in the adult subjects. Ten adult subjects walked at slower treadmill speeds ( km/h), and another 10 adult subjects walked at faster treadmill speeds ( km/h). All children were tested at slower treadmill speeds ( km/h). The treadmill speeds for individuals were set to match cadence between adults and children. Subjects performed a total of 7 blocks that each consisted of 100 steps (Fig. 1C). In random blocks, the targets appeared randomly at locations that required different step lengths (i.e., short, medium, long). In sequence blocks, subjects were presented with a repeating sequence of step lengths (i.e., short-long-medium-long-short-medium). The first random block (R1) was used to familiarize the subject to the task. The second random block (R2) provided a measure of final baseline performance. In subsequent training blocks (S1 S3), subjects were presented with the repeating sequence. Sequence-specific learning was calculated as the difference in performance between the last training block (S3) and the last random block (R3); nonspecific learning was calculated as the difference between blocks R2 and R3. The final block (S4) tested whether subjects could immediately recall the learned sequence after reexposure to the random sequence (R3). At the end, subjects were asked whether they noticed any patterns or repeating sequence? Sequence-by-sequence success rate was calculated as the fraction of hits within each six-step sequence. The score (number of successful hits) and mean error were calculated to measure performance within each block. Error was defined as the absolute difference between the by on November 24, 2017

3 2016 LOCOMOTOR SEQUENCE LEARNING foot position and target position (i.e., center of the circle center of the square ) and was normalized to each subject s leg length. Error in the anterior-posterior directions was analyzed. To evaluate sequencespecific learning, we subtracted the mean score in R3 from the mean score in the S3 block (Perez et al. 2007; Willingham et al. 2000). Nonspecific learning was measured by subtracting the mean score in R3 from the mean score in R2 (Perez et al. 2007). Statistical analysis. Between-groups repeated-measures ANOVA was used to compare performance changes over the experimental blocks between groups (e.g., 7 blocks 2 age groups) and speed conditions (e.g., 7 blocks 2 speeds). When ANOVAs showed significant effects, the Tukey s honest significant difference test was used for post hoc pairwise analysis (i.e., R3 and S3 for sequencespecific learning, R2 and R3 for nonspecific learning). RESULTS Locomotor sequence learning. Subjects improved their score over the seven blocks of testing, performing better in sequence blocks compared with random blocks. A typical subject hit about 50% of the targets in the first random block, R1, and improved his score on the second random block, R2, when he became more familiar with the task (Fig. 2A). Subjects had more difficulty shortening than lengthening step length. The number of hits on short steps was significantly lower compared Fig. 2. Locomotor sequence learning. Each sequence consisted of 6 steps, and each block consisted of 16 complete sequences. A: performance in a typical subject over 7 blocks. Score (left y-axis, maximum score 100) is the total number of hits in each block (top x-axis). Success rate (right y-axis) is the fraction of hits within each 6-step sequence (bottom x-axis). Score (bar graph) and success rate (line graph) for the same subject are overlaid on the plot to show variability within each block. B: mean success rate for each sequence averaged across subjects (n 10). Shaded area of the curve represents standard error (SE). C: mean score (number of hits) over 7 blocks of testing averaged across subjects (n 10). D: nonspecific learning (R3 R2) and sequence-specific learning (S3 R3) for individual subjects. E: group average mean error (n 10) in the anterior-posterior (AP) direction, calculated as the foot position (5th MT) relative to the target position. Error was normalized to each subject s leg length. F: group average cadence (n 10). *P 0.05; ns, not significant. with long steps (P 0.001). Learning continued over the subsequent sequence blocks, S1 S3, as the score improved further. To determine whether the learning was sequence specific, we exposed the subject to another random block, R3. A drop in score in R3 compared with S3 indicates that the learning was sequence specific. That is, the subject did not simply learn to react to the visual stimuli. Moreover, the subject was able to recall the learned sequence on reexposure in S4. We also quantified sequence-by-sequence performance as the rate of success within each six-step sequence. Sequenceby-sequence performance is plotted on top of the score from the same subject to show variability in performance within a block (Fig. 2A). Learning was evident in both the average sequence-bysequence success rate (Fig. 2B) and the average change in score across subjects (Fig. 2C). Statistical analysis was performed on the change in score across blocks. Significant performance changes were observed over the seven blocks of testing [repeated-measures ANOVA: effect of block, F(6,54) 28.7, P 0.001]. Importantly, the score decreased from S3 to R3, indicating that the learning was sequence specific (P 0.03). Moreover, the amount of nonspecific learning (i.e., R3 R2) was not significant (P 0.1), sug-

4 LOCOMOTOR SEQUENCE LEARNING 2017 gesting that improved performance (after becoming accustomed to the walking task in R1) was mostly accounted for by sequence-specific learning. The amounts of nonspecific and sequence-specific learning for individual subjects are illustrated in Fig. 2D. At the end, subjects were asked whether they noticed any patterns or repeating sequence? None of the subjects gave perfect descriptions of the six-step repeating sequence. Thus the step length sequence appears to be learned without conscious awareness. The group average (n 10) mean error is plotted in Fig. 2E. The magnitude of errors decreased as the score improved [repeated-measures ANOVA: F(6,54) 16.8, P 0.001; Fig. 2E]. Post hoc analysis showed a significant decrease in error from R1 to R2 (P 0.001). The same cadence was maintained across all blocks [repeated-measures ANOVA: F(6,54) 0.2, P 0.9; Fig. 2F]. Younger children performed worse in baseline but showed the same amount of sequence learning compared with older children. To compare the learning rates from early to late childhood, children were divided into two age groups: 6 10 and yr old (Fig. 3, A and B). The groups reflect the development in cortical control of gait, which is not fully mature in children under the age of 10 yr (Petersen et al. 2010). Figure 3A shows the average sequence-by-sequence success rate across the two age groups. Younger children had lower success rate in baseline (R1, R2), but they were able to improve performance over each block of training. Children in both age groups showed similar success rates by the third sequence block, S3. Statistical analysis was performed on the group average change in score (Fig. 3B). The results showed a significant difference between age groups [repeated-measures ANOVA: F(2,15) 6.01, P 0.02; block age, F(12,150) 0.9, P 0.5]. Post hoc analysis showed a significant difference between younger and older children (P 0.001) and between younger children and adults (P 0.001). All age groups (6 10 yr, yr, adults) showed sequence-specific learning (P 0.001), as indicated by a decrease in score from S3 to R3. There was no increase in score from R3 to R2 (P 0.5), which indicates little nonspecific learning beyond R2. The magnitude of error was significantly reduced over the seven blocks of training [repeated-measures ANOVA: effect of blocks, F(6,144) 32.5, P 0.001]. Error was normalized to each subject s leg length; the average leg length is Downloaded from by on November 24, 2017 Fig. 3. Locomotor sequence learning in healthy adults vs. children. A: mean success rate for each sequence averaged across children in two age groups: 6 10 yr (n 8) and yr (n 10). Shaded area of the curve represents SE. B D: group average mean score (B), normalized AP error (C), and cadence (D). Bars represent average data across 7 blocks of testing within each age group: 6 10 yr (n 8), yr (n 10), and adults (n 10). E G: individual data for each child plotted against age. Pearson s correlation (r) is shown for baseline score (E), nonspecific learning (F) and sequence-specific learning (G). *P 0.05.

5 2018 LOCOMOTOR SEQUENCE LEARNING m in younger children, m in older children, and m in adults. The normalized errors were largest in younger children [repeated-measures ANOVA: effect of age, F(2,24) 21.0, P 0.001; blocks age, F(12,144) 2.2, P 0.01; Fig. 3C]. Post hoc analysis showed a significant difference in normalized error between younger and older children (P 0.001) and between younger children and adults (P 0.001). Subjects maintained the same cadence across all blocks [repeated-measures ANOVA: effect of blocks, F(6,150) 1.0, P 0.4; Fig. 3D]. Note that cadence was matched across age groups [repeated-measures ANOVA: effect of age, F(2,25) 0.07, P 0.8]. Regression analysis of individual data was performed to confirm the results from group average data. The baseline (R2) score for each child is plotted against age (Fig. 3E). There was a significant correlation between age and baseline score (r 0.71, P 0.001); performance improved from early to late childhood. There was no significant correlation between age and nonspecific learning (Fig. 3F; r 0.22, P 0.37) or sequence-specific learning (Fig. 3G; r 0.05, P 0.85). Thus younger children were able to learn and improve performance, despite starting with lower baseline score compared with older children. Learning capacity is dependent on walking speed. Locomotor patterns are characterized by distinct regularity in spatial and temporal parameters; the percentage of stride time spent in stance phase decreases as walking speed increases (Dietz et al. 1994). At faster walking speeds, subjects have less time to increase (or decrease) push-off forces to shorten (or lengthen) a step. Therefore, we predicted that subjects would perform worse during fast walking compared with slow walking. Figure 4, A and B, shows the performance of adults walking Fig. 4. Locomotor sequence learning at different walking speeds. A: mean success rate for each sequence averaged across subjects for two walking speeds: slow ( km/h) and fast ( km/h). B D: group average mean score (B), normalized AP error (C), and cadence (D). Bars represent average data across 7 blocks of testing at 2 different walking speeds: slow (n 10) and fast (n 10). *P at two different speeds. The score was lower at the fast walking speed [repeated-measures ANOVA: effect of speed, F(1,18) 19.1, P 0.001]. The score improved over seven blocks of testing at both walking speeds [repeated-measures ANOVA: effect of block, F(6,108) 46.7, P 0.001; block speed, F(6,108) 1.2, P 0.3]. Post hoc analysis showed a significant amount of sequence-specific learning (P 0.01) at both speeds. There was no significant difference between blocks R3 and R2 (P 0.1), indicating little nonspecific learning beyond the first random block R2. Subjects reached a lower final score at the fast speed than at the slow speed, which suggests that learning capacity may be more limited when the task becomes more challenging. Subjects made larger errors at the fast walking speed [repeatedmeasures ANOVA: effect of speed, F(1,18) 539.6, P 0.001; Fig. 4C]. The magnitude of the error decreased over seven blocks of training [repeated-measures ANOVA: effect of block, F(6,108) 25.8, P 0.001; block speed, F(6,108) 3.4, P 0.004]. Cadence was higher at the fast walking speed [repeated-measures ANOVA: effect of speed, F(1,18) 18,285.6, P 0.001; Fig. 4D]. This is expected, because the same step length was imposed at the slow and fast walking speeds. Subjects used the same cadence across the seven blocks of testing [repeated-measures ANOVA: effect of block, F(6,108) 0.2, P 0.9]. DISCUSSION We used a novel paradigm to study spatial sequence learning during walking. The results showed that visually guided step length modification was improved through training. After training, subjects performed better on the repeating sequence compared with random sequences, suggesting that subjects

6 LOCOMOTOR SEQUENCE LEARNING 2019 used knowledge about the sequence to plan and control foot placement (rather than simply reacting to the visual stimuli). Moreover, none of the subjects were able to describe the repeating sequence correctly, suggesting that the step length sequence could be learned implicitly without conscious awareness. Visuomotor coordination in locomotion. Vision provides information about the position of objects within the environment and our own movement within the environment. The nervous system makes anticipatory adjustments to the control of walking in response to visual cues (Matthis and Fajen 2014; Warren et al. 1986). Patla et al. (1989) have reported the effects of the timing of visual cues to regulate step length during overground walking. Subjects were able to shorten step length when the visual cue was given before the contralateral heel contact, but not when the visual cue was given after the contralateral heel contact (Patla et al. 1989). When the visual cue was given at or before the contralateral heel contact, subjects had enough time to regulate step length by changing the contralateral push-off force. Subjects reduced push-off force to shorten step length and increased push-off force to increase step length (Patla et al. 1989). Consistent with previous reports on step length modification (Patla et al. 1989), subjects had more difficulty shortening step length during our visuomotor walking paradigm. Moreover, we found that subjects were less accurate at the fast walking speeds because there is less time to change push-off forces to shorten (or lengthen) a step. Locomotor sequence learning. We applied principles of sensorimotor learning to establish a new paradigm for studying visually guided sequence learning during walking. Learning can be defined as the storage of new motor patterns through repeated practice. Once a movement is learned, it could be immediately used in the appropriate context. Motor sequence learning involves integrating a series of movements that are executed in a specific order. The SRT task from the original study by Nissen and Bullemer (1987) has been used to study age-related changes in this type of procedural learning (Janacsek et al. 2012; King et al. 2013; Thomas et al. 2004). However, there are no studies in the literature that have explored sequence learning in lower limb movements during walking. We found that healthy adults and children improved their performance over 7 blocks of testing and learned a specific walking sequence over 300 training steps. In this case, improved performance and accuracy were achieved without changing movement timing (e.g., cadence). This demonstrates that sequence learning can be integrated with human locomotor pattern generation. Neural control of gait modifications. Visually guided limb modifications must be integrated into the locomotor pattern during ongoing movements (Georgopoulos and Grillner 1989; Rossignol 1996). Cortical mechanisms are involved in the planning and execution of movements during visually guided walking (Drew et al. 2008; Drew and Marigold 2015). The motor cortex of cats is not strongly modulated during unobstructed level walking but becomes more active when precise adjustments in paw placement or end-point control is required (Beloozerova and Sirota 1993; Drew 1993). Lesions of the pyramidal tract or the corticospinal tract can cause marked deficits in voluntary modifications to the walking pattern, such as in obstacle avoidance (Drew et al. 2002). In humans, the corticospinal tract directly modulates leg muscle activation during steady-state walking (Petersen et al. 2001, 2010). Increase in muscular coherence in developing children reflects the maturation level of the corticospinal drive (Petersen et al. 2010). In addition, corticospinal input facilitates ankle muscle activation during foot placements in visual walking tasks (Schubert et al. 1999). Thus the human corticospinal tract likely contributes to the precision of foot placement control during human walking. Locomotor patterns in young children (age 4 yr) develop rapidly, resulting in greater specificity in muscle activation patterns on foot contact (Dominici et al. 2011). More gradual development in walking occurs until 8 10 yr of age (Lacquaniti et al. 2012). In general, the walking pattern of children shows greater variability compared with that of adults (Dominici et al. 2010; Petersen et al. 2010). The corticospinal drive is not fully mature in children under the age of 10 yr (Petersen et al. 2010). We therefore predicted that visuomotor control of walking might be more difficult in younger children compared with adults. Our results are consistent with the hypothesis that the development of the corticospinal tract contributes to the control of foot placement. Children up to 10 yr of age performed worse at the task compared with older children. Interesting, we found that the ability to learn in healthy children aged 6 15 yr is comparable to that of healthy adults during visually guided step length modifications. There was no correlation in the amount of sequence-specific learning and nonspecific learning with age. We speculate that this may be related to participation in sports and other activities, which often involve changes in step length (e.g., soccer, hopscotch). The result is interesting because it suggests that mechanisms that underlie the planning and execution of voluntary gait modifications are intact before the locomotor system is fully mature. The ability to make gait modifications may facilitate the learning of new walking patterns. In fact, our results showed that the magnitude and rate of locomotor sequence learning are similar between children and adults. Conclusions. In summary, we have developed and used a novel paradigm to study sequence learning during visually guided walking in healthy subjects. Our results demonstrate that implicit sequence learning can be integrated with a highly automatic task such as walking. We conclude that visuomotor coordination in walking is dependent on the development of the corticospinal tract in children. GRANTS This work was supported by the Danish Medical Research Council ( /FSS) and the Whitaker International Program. DISCLOSURES No conflicts of interest, financial or otherwise, are declared by the authors. AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS J.T.C. and J.B.N. conception and design of research; J.T.C. and P.J. performed experiments; J.T.C. and P.J. analyzed data; J.T.C., P.J., and J.B.N. interpreted results of experiments; J.T.C. prepared figures; J.T.C. drafted manuscript; J.T.C., P.J., and J.B.N. edited and revised manuscript; J.T.C., P.J., and J.B.N. approved final version of manuscript.

7 2020 LOCOMOTOR SEQUENCE LEARNING REFERENCES Andujar JE, Lajoie K, Drew T. A contribution of area 5 of the posterior parietal cortex to the planning of visually guided locomotion: limb-specific and limb-independent effects. J Neurophysiol 103: , Beloozerova IN, Sirota MG. The role of the motor cortex in the control of vigour of locomotor movements in the cat. J Physiol 461: 27 46, Cohen A, Ivry RI, Keele SW. Attention and structure in sequence learning. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 16: 17 30, Curran T, Keele SW. Attentional and nonattentional forms of sequence learning. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 19: , Dietz V, Zijlstra W, Duysens J. Human neuronal interlimb coordination during split-belt locomotion. Exp Brain Res 101: , Dominici N, Ivanenko YP, Cappellini G, d Avella A, Mondi V, Cicchese M, Fabiano A, Silei T, Di Paolo A, Giannini C, Poppele RE, Lacquaniti F. Locomotor primitives in newborn babies and their development. Science 334: , Dominici N, Ivanenko YP, Cappellini G, Zampagni ML, Lacquaniti F. Kinematic strategies in newly walking toddlers stepping over different support surfaces. J Neurophysiol 103: , Drew T. Motor cortical activity during voluntary gait modifications in the cat. I. Cells related to the forelimbs. J Neurophysiol 70: , Drew T, Andujar JE, Lajoie K, Yakovenko S. Cortical mechanisms involved in visuomotor coordination during precision walking. Brain Res Rev 57: , Drew T, Jiang W, Widajewicz W. Contributions of the motor cortex to the control of the hindlimbs during locomotion in the cat. Brain Res Rev 40: , Drew T, Marigold DS. Taking the next step: cortical contributions to the control of locomotion. Curr Opin Neurobiol 33C: 25 33, Erni T, Dietz V. Obstacle avoidance during human walking: learning rate and cross-modal transfer. J Physiol 534: , Georgopoulos AP, Grillner S. Visuomotor coordination in reaching and locomotion. Science 245: , Janacsek K, Fiser J, Nemeth D. The best time to acquire new skills: age-related differences in implicit sequence learning across the human lifespan. Dev Sci 15: , King BR, Fogel SM, Albouy G, Doyon J. Neural correlates of the age-related changes in motor sequence learning and motor adaptation in older adults. Front Hum Neurosci 7: 142, Lacquaniti F, Ivanenko YP, Zago M. Development of human locomotion. Curr Opin Neurobiol 22: , Lajoie K, Bloomfield LW, Nelson FJ, Suh JJ, Marigold DS. The contribution of vision, proprioception, and efference copy in storing a neural representation for guiding trail leg trajectory over an obstacle. J Neurophysiol 107: , Lajoie K, Drew T. Lesions of area 5 of the posterior parietal cortex in the cat produce errors in the accuracy of paw placement during visually guided locomotion. J Neurophysiol 97: , Matthis JS, Fajen BR. Visual control of foot placement when walking over complex terrain. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 40: , Nissen MJ, Bullemer P. Attentional requirements of learning: Evidence from performance measures. Cogn Psychol 19: 1 32, Patla AE, Robinson C, Samways M, Armstrong CJ. Visual control of step length during overground locomotion task-specific modulation of the locomotor synergy. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 15: , Perez MA, Wise SP, Willingham DT, Cohen LG. Neurophysiological mechanisms involved in transfer of procedural knowledge. J Neurosci 27: , Petersen NT, Butler JE, Marchand-Pauvert V, Fisher R, Ledebt A, Pyndt HS, Hansen NL, Nielsen JB. Suppression of EMG activity by transcranial magnetic stimulation in human subjects during walking. J Physiol 537: , Petersen TH, Kliim-Due M, Farmer SF, Nielsen JB. Childhood development of common drive to a human leg muscle during ankle dorsiflexion and gait. J Physiol 588: , Rossignol S. Visuomotor regulation of locomotion. Can J Physiol Pharmacol 74: , Schubert M, Curt A, Colombo G, Berger W, Dietz V. Voluntary control of human gait: conditioning of magnetically evoked motor responses in a precision stepping task. Exp Brain Res 126: , Spaulding SJ, Barber B, Colby M, Cormack B, Mick T, Jenkins ME. Cueing and gait improvement among people with Parkinson s disease: a meta-analysis. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 94: , Thomas KM, Hunt RH, Vizueta N, Sommer T, Durston S, Yang YH, Worden MS. Evidence of developmental differences in implicit sequence learning: an fmri study of children and adults. J Cogn Neurosci 16: , Warren WH Jr, Young DS, Lee DN. Visual control of step length during running over irregular terrain. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 12: , Willingham DB, Wells LA, Farrell JM, Stemwedel ME. Implicit motor sequence learning is represented in response locations. Mem Cognit 28: , 2000.

Natural error patterns enable transfer of motor learning to novel contexts

Natural error patterns enable transfer of motor learning to novel contexts J Neurophysiol 107: 346 356, 2012. First published September 28, 2011; doi:10.1152/jn.00570.2011. Natural error patterns enable transfer of motor learning to novel contexts Gelsy Torres-Oviedo and Amy

More information

Transformation of nonfunctional spinal circuits into functional states after the loss of brain input

Transformation of nonfunctional spinal circuits into functional states after the loss of brain input Transformation of nonfunctional spinal circuits into functional states after the loss of brain input G. Courtine, Y. P. Gerasimenko, R. van den Brand, A. Yew, P. Musienko, H. Zhong, B. Song, Y. Ao, R.

More information

The Influence of Load Carrying Modes on Gait variables of Healthy Indian Women

The Influence of Load Carrying Modes on Gait variables of Healthy Indian Women The Influence of Load Carrying Modes on Gait variables of Healthy Indian Women *Guha Thakurta A, Iqbal R and De A National Institute of Industrial Engineering, Powai, Vihar Lake, Mumbai-400087, India,

More information

Increased central common drive to ankle plantar flexor and dorsiflexor muscles during visually guided gait

Increased central common drive to ankle plantar flexor and dorsiflexor muscles during visually guided gait ORIGINAL RESEARCH Physiological Reports ISSN 2051-817X Increased central common drive to ankle plantar flexor and dorsiflexor muscles during visually guided gait Peter Jensen 1, Nicole Jacqueline Jensen

More information

Interlimb Coordination During Locomotion: What Can be Adapted and Stored?

Interlimb Coordination During Locomotion: What Can be Adapted and Stored? J Neurophysiol 94: 2403 2415, 2005. First published June 15, 2005; doi:10.1152/jn.00089.2005. Interlimb Coordination During Locomotion: What Can be Adapted and Stored? Darcy S. Reisman, 1,2 Hannah J. Block,

More information

Sensitivity of toe clearance to leg joint angles during extensive practice of obstacle crossing: Effects of vision and task goal

Sensitivity of toe clearance to leg joint angles during extensive practice of obstacle crossing: Effects of vision and task goal Sensitivity of toe clearance to leg joint angles during extensive practice of obstacle crossing: Effects of vision and task goal Sérgio Tosi Rodrigues 1, Valéria Duarte Garcia 1,2, Arturo Forner- Cordero

More information

Walking. Zachary Zeidler Graduate Program in Neuroscience University of Minnesota

Walking. Zachary Zeidler Graduate Program in Neuroscience University of Minnesota Walking Zachary Zeidler Graduate Program in Neuroscience University of Minnesota 1 Locomotion Gaits Walking Running Skipping Hopping Trotting 2 Notice the: - Rhythmic pattern of limb activation while walking

More information

Adaptive Locomotion Controller for a Quadruped Robot

Adaptive Locomotion Controller for a Quadruped Robot Adaptive Locomotion Controller for a Quadruped Robot Step 1: Model of sensory feedback in animals during locomotion Simon Ruffieux 07 1 Introduction Sensory feedback is an important component of locomotion

More information

Supplementary Figure S1

Supplementary Figure S1 Supplementary Figure S1: Anterior and posterior views of the marker set used in the running gait trials. Forty-six markers were attached to the subject (15 markers on each leg, 4 markers on each arm, and

More information

Transfer of Motor Performance in an Obstacle Avoidance Task to Different Walking Conditions

Transfer of Motor Performance in an Obstacle Avoidance Task to Different Walking Conditions J Neurophysiol 92: 2010 2016, 2004; 10.1152/jn.00397.2004. Transfer of Motor Performance in an Obstacle Avoidance Task to Different Walking Conditions Tania Lam and Volker Dietz Spinal Cord Injury Center,

More information

intended velocity ( u k arm movements

intended velocity ( u k arm movements Fig. A Complete Brain-Machine Interface B Human Subjects Closed-Loop Simulator ensemble action potentials (n k ) ensemble action potentials (n k ) primary motor cortex simulated primary motor cortex neuroprosthetic

More information

Muscle synergies during locomotion in the cat: a model for motor cortex control

Muscle synergies during locomotion in the cat: a model for motor cortex control J Physiol 586.5 (2008) pp 1239 1245 1239 SYMPOSIUM REPORT Muscle synergies during locomotion in the cat: a model for motor cortex control Trevor Drew 1,2, John Kalaska 1,2 and Nedialko Krouchev 2 1 Groupe

More information

EXSC 408L Fall '03 Problem Set #2 Linear Motion. Linear Motion

EXSC 408L Fall '03 Problem Set #2 Linear Motion. Linear Motion Problems: 1. Once you have recorded the calibration frame for a data collection, why is it important to make sure the camera does not shut off? hat happens if the camera automatically shuts off after being

More information

Neurorehabil Neural Repair Oct 23. [Epub ahead of print]

Neurorehabil Neural Repair Oct 23. [Epub ahead of print] APPENDICE Neurorehabil Neural Repair. 2009 Oct 23. [Epub ahead of print] Segmental Muscle Vibration Improves Walking in Chronic Stroke Patients With Foot Drop: A Randomized Controlled Trial. Paoloni M,

More information

INITIATING NORMAL WALKING OF A DYNAMIC BIPED WITH A BIOLOGICALLY MOTIVATED CONTROL

INITIATING NORMAL WALKING OF A DYNAMIC BIPED WITH A BIOLOGICALLY MOTIVATED CONTROL 1 INITIATING NORMAL WALKING OF A DYNAMIC BIPED WITH A BIOLOGICALLY MOTIVATED CONTROL T. LUKSCH and K. BERNS Robotics Research Lab, University of Kaiserslautern Kaiserslautern, Germany E-mail: luksch@informatik.uni-kl.de

More information

Modular organization across changing task demands in healthy and poststroke gait

Modular organization across changing task demands in healthy and poststroke gait ORIGINAL RESEARCH Physiological Reports ISSN 2051-817X Modular organization across changing task demands in healthy and poststroke gait Rebecca L. Routson 1, Steven A. Kautz 2,3 & Richard R. Neptune 1

More information

Gait Analyser. Description of Walking Performance

Gait Analyser. Description of Walking Performance Gait Analyser Description of Walking Performance This brochure will help you to understand clearly the parameters described in the report of the Gait Analyser, provide you with tips to implement the walking

More information

ASSISTED AND RESISTED METHODS FOR SPEED DEVELOPMENT (PART 1)

ASSISTED AND RESISTED METHODS FOR SPEED DEVELOPMENT (PART 1) ASSISTED AND RESISTED METHODS FOR SPEED DEVELOPMENT (PART 1) By Adrian Faccioni Adrian Faccioni, a lecturer at the Centre of Sports Studies, University of Canberra, Australia, presents a detailed evaluation

More information

Citation for published version (APA): Otter, A. R. D. (2005). Gait control after stroke: a neuromuscular approach to functional recovery s.n.

Citation for published version (APA): Otter, A. R. D. (2005). Gait control after stroke: a neuromuscular approach to functional recovery s.n. University of Groningen Gait control after stroke den Otter, Rob IMPORTANT NOTE: You are advised to consult the publisher's version (publisher's PDF) if you wish to cite from it. Please check the document

More information

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Gait Posture. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2009 May 1.

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Gait Posture. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2009 May 1. NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Published in final edited form as: Gait Posture. 2008 May ; 27(4): 710 714. Two simple methods for determining gait events during treadmill and overground walking using

More information

Spasticity in gait. Wessex ACPIN Spasticity Presentation Alison Clarke

Spasticity in gait. Wessex ACPIN Spasticity Presentation Alison Clarke Spasticity in gait Clinicians recognise spasticity but the elements of spasticity contributing to gait patterns are often difficult to identify: Variability of muscle tone Observation/recording General

More information

Adaptation reveals independent control networks for human walking

Adaptation reveals independent control networks for human walking Adaptation reveals independent control networks for human walking Julia T Choi 1,2 & Amy J Bastian 1,3 Human walking is remarkably adaptable on short and long timescales. We can immediately transition

More information

Equine Cannon Angle System

Equine Cannon Angle System Equine Cannon System How to interpret the results December 2010 Page 1 of 14 Table of Contents Introduction... 3 The Sagittal Plane... 4 The Coronal Plane... 5 Results Format... 6 How to Interpret the

More information

INTERACTION OF STEP LENGTH AND STEP RATE DURING SPRINT RUNNING

INTERACTION OF STEP LENGTH AND STEP RATE DURING SPRINT RUNNING INTERACTION OF STEP LENGTH AND STEP RATE DURING SPRINT RUNNING Joseph P. Hunter 1, Robert N. Marshall 1,, and Peter J. McNair 3 1 Department of Sport and Exercise Science, The University of Auckland, Auckland,

More information

Running from injury 2

Running from injury 2 Created as a free resource by Clinical Edge Based on Physio Edge podcast 049 with Dr Rich Willy Get your free trial of online Physio education by clicking here Running injuries & assessment Running assessment

More information

ASSESMENT Introduction REPORTS Running Reports Walking Reports Written Report

ASSESMENT Introduction REPORTS Running Reports Walking Reports Written Report ASSESMENT REPORTS Introduction Left panel Avatar Playback Right Panel Patient Gait Parameters Report Tab Click on parameter to view avatar at that point in time 2 Introduction Software will compare gait

More information

Biomechanical analysis of gait termination in year old youth at preferred and fast walking speeds

Biomechanical analysis of gait termination in year old youth at preferred and fast walking speeds Brigham Young University BYU ScholarsArchive All Faculty Publications 2016-10 Biomechanical analysis of gait termination in 11 17 year old youth at preferred and fast walking speeds Sarah T. Ridge Brigham

More information

premise that interdependent body systems (e.g. musculoskeletal, motor, sensory, and cognitive

premise that interdependent body systems (e.g. musculoskeletal, motor, sensory, and cognitive APPENDIX 2 Motor Control Intervention Protocol The dynamic systems approach underlying motor control intervention is based on the premise that interdependent body systems (e.g. musculoskeletal, motor,

More information

RUNNING SHOE STIFFNESS: THE EFFECT ON WALKING GAIT

RUNNING SHOE STIFFNESS: THE EFFECT ON WALKING GAIT RUNNING SHOE STIFFNESS: THE EFFECT ON WALKING GAIT Stephen N Stanley, Peter J M c Nair, Angela G Walker, & Robert N Marshall Auckland Institute of Technology, Auckland, New Zealand University of Auckland,

More information

An investigation of kinematic and kinetic variables for the description of prosthetic gait using the ENOCH system

An investigation of kinematic and kinetic variables for the description of prosthetic gait using the ENOCH system An investigation of kinematic and kinetic variables for the description of prosthetic gait using the ENOCH system K. OBERG and H. LANSHAMMAR* Amputee Training and Research Unit, University Hospital, Fack,

More information

KASAMATSU VERSUS TSUKAHARA VAULT

KASAMATSU VERSUS TSUKAHARA VAULT KASAMATSU VERSUS TSUKAHARA VAULT Yoshie Motoshima 1, Akira Maeda 2 1 Graduate School of Physical Education, National Institute of Fitness and Sports in Kanoya, Japan 2 National Institute of Fitness and

More information

The Effects of Simulated Knee Arthrodesis and Temporal Acclimation on Gait Kinematics

The Effects of Simulated Knee Arthrodesis and Temporal Acclimation on Gait Kinematics The Effects of Simulated Knee Arthrodesis and Temporal Acclimation on Gait Kinematics Eric M. Lucas, MS 1, Randy Hutchison, PhD 2, Justin Marro, MS 1, Taylor Gambon 1, John D. DesJardins, PhD 1. 1 Clemson

More information

VISUOMOTOR CONTROL OF STRAIGHT AND BREAKING GOLF PUTTS 1

VISUOMOTOR CONTROL OF STRAIGHT AND BREAKING GOLF PUTTS 1 Perceptual and Motor Skills, 2009, 109, 555-562. Perceptual and Motor Skills 2009 VISUOMOTOR CONTROL OF STRAIGHT AND BREAKING GOLF PUTTS 1 MARK R. WILSON AND RICHARD C. PEARCY University of Exeter Summary.

More information

Gait. Kinesiology RHS 341 Lecture 12 Dr. Einas Al-Eisa

Gait. Kinesiology RHS 341 Lecture 12 Dr. Einas Al-Eisa Gait Kinesiology RHS 341 Lecture 12 Dr. Einas Al-Eisa Definitions Locomotion = the act of moving from one place to the other Gait = the manner of walking Definitions Walking = a smooth, highly coordinated,

More information

STATIC AND DYNAMIC EVALUATION OF THE DRIVER SPEED PERCEPTION AND SELECTION PROCESS

STATIC AND DYNAMIC EVALUATION OF THE DRIVER SPEED PERCEPTION AND SELECTION PROCESS STATIC AND DYNAMIC EVALUATION OF THE DRIVER SPEED PERCEPTION AND SELECTION PROCESS David S. Hurwitz, Michael A. Knodler, Jr. University of Massachusetts Amherst Department of Civil & Environmental Engineering

More information

An investigation of lower-extremity functional asymmetry for nonpreferred able-bodied walking speeds

An investigation of lower-extremity functional asymmetry for nonpreferred able-bodied walking speeds Original Research An investigation of lower-extremity functional asymmetry for nonpreferred able-bodied walking speeds JOHN RICE* and MATTHEW K. SEELEY Department of Exercise Sciences; 106 SFH; Brigham

More information

Efficacy of Static and Dynamic Distance Perception on Kumite Performance in Karate

Efficacy of Static and Dynamic Distance Perception on Kumite Performance in Karate World Journal of Sport Sciences 3 (S): 205-209, 2010 ISSN 2078-4724 IDOSI Publications, 2010 Efficacy of Static and Dynamic Distance Perception on Kumite Performance in Karate Ahmed M. Abd El-Kader Department

More information

Impact of target selection on front kick kinematics in taekwondo pilot study

Impact of target selection on front kick kinematics in taekwondo pilot study Physical Activity Review vol. 4, 2016 Original Articles DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.16926/par.2016.04.07 Impact of target selection on front kick kinematics in taekwondo pilot study Authors' Contribution:

More information

Adjustments of Speed and Path when Avoiding Collisions with Another Pedestrian

Adjustments of Speed and Path when Avoiding Collisions with Another Pedestrian Adjustments of Speed and Path when Avoiding Collisions with Another Pedestrian Markus Huber 1, Yi-Huang Su 2, Melanie Krüger 2,3, Katrin Faschian 2, Stefan Glasauer 1,4,5 *, Joachim Hermsdörfer 2 1 Center

More information

Walk - Run Activity --An S and P Wave Travel Time Simulation ( S minus P Earthquake Location Method)

Walk - Run Activity --An S and P Wave Travel Time Simulation ( S minus P Earthquake Location Method) Walk - Run Activity --An S and P Wave Travel Time Simulation ( S minus P Earthquake Location Method) L. W. Braile and S. J. Braile (June, 2000) braile@purdue.edu http://web.ics.purdue.edu/~braile Walk

More information

Today: the first activity system. Neural Control of Movement LOCOMOTION GAIT DESCRIPTIONS. Review: that amazing spinal cord! What we do: gait patterns

Today: the first activity system. Neural Control of Movement LOCOMOTION GAIT DESCRIPTIONS. Review: that amazing spinal cord! What we do: gait patterns Neural Control of Movement LOCOMOTION Today: the first activity system Review: that amazing spinal cord! What we do: gait patterns How we do it: neural circuitry and the role of sensory feedback and higher

More information

Motor function analysis: from animal models to patients

Motor function analysis: from animal models to patients Motor function analysis: from animal models to patients ZNZ lecture 27.04.2016 Linard Filli Sensorimotor Lab University Hospital Zurich Linard.Filli@usz.ch Content of lecture Basic neuroanatomy of motor

More information

Performance & Motor Control Characteristics of Functional Skill. Part III: Throwing, Catching & Hitting

Performance & Motor Control Characteristics of Functional Skill. Part III: Throwing, Catching & Hitting Performance & Motor Control Characteristics of Functional Skill Part III: Throwing, Catching & Hitting Throwing Interesting Facts Studies indicate that boys move across the stages at a faster rate than

More information

Medicine Meets Virtual Reality 21

Medicine Meets Virtual Reality 21 University of Nebraska Omaha DigitalCommons@UNO Faculty Books and Monographs 2014 Medicine Meets Virtual Reality 21 James D. Westwood Susan W. Westwood Li Felländer-Tsai Cali M. Fidopiastis Randy S. Haluck

More information

SPRINTING CHARACTERISTICS OF WOMEN S 100 METER FINALS AT THE IAAF WORLD CHAMPIONSHOPS DAEGU 2011

SPRINTING CHARACTERISTICS OF WOMEN S 100 METER FINALS AT THE IAAF WORLD CHAMPIONSHOPS DAEGU 2011 9:30-9:45 am Sang-Kyoon Park. Sprinting characteristics of women's 100 meter finals at the IAAF world championships Daegu 2011. (228) SPRINTING CHARACTERISTICS OF WOMEN S 100 METER FINALS AT THE IAAF WORLD

More information

Complex movement patterns of a bipedal walk

Complex movement patterns of a bipedal walk 1 Complex movement patterns of a bipedal walk Objectives After completing this lesson, you will be able to: Describe the complex movement patterns of a bipedal walk. Describe the biomechanics of walking

More information

The effect of different backpack loading systems on trunk forward lean angle during walking among college students

The effect of different backpack loading systems on trunk forward lean angle during walking among college students Available online at www.scholarsresearchlibrary.com European Journal of Sports and Exercise Science, 2012, 1 (1):1-5 (http://scholarsresearchlibrary.com/archive.html) ISSN: 2278 005X The effect of different

More information

Nervous System: Reaction Time Student Version

Nervous System: Reaction Time Student Version Nervous System: Reaction Time Student Version Key Concepts: Your nervous system allows your body to react to different stimuli (external events) Reactions can be voluntary (eg: swinging a bat at a ball

More information

Artifacts Due to Filtering Mismatch in Drop Landing Moment Data

Artifacts Due to Filtering Mismatch in Drop Landing Moment Data Camenga et al. UW-L Journal of Undergraduate Research XVI (213) Artifacts Due to Filtering Mismatch in Drop Landing Moment Data Elizabeth T. Camenga, Casey J. Rutten, Brendan D. Gould, Jillian T. Asmus,

More information

EFFECTS OF SPEED AND INCLINE ON LOWER EXTREMITY KINEMATICS DURING TREADMILL JOGGING IN HEALTHY SUBJECTS

EFFECTS OF SPEED AND INCLINE ON LOWER EXTREMITY KINEMATICS DURING TREADMILL JOGGING IN HEALTHY SUBJECTS BIOMEDICAL ENGINEERING- EFFECTS OF SPEED AND INCLINE ON LOWER EXTREMITY KINEMATICS DURING TREADMILL JOGGING IN HEALTHY SUBJECTS 73 LAN-YUEN GUO 1, FONG-CHIN SU 2, CHICH-HAUNG YANG 3, SHU-HUI WANG 3, JYH-JONG

More information

The Journal of Physiology Neuroscience

The Journal of Physiology Neuroscience J Physiol 59.4 (23) pp 8 95 8 The Journal of Physiology Neuroscience Learning to be economical: the energy cost of walking tracks motor adaptation James M. Finley,2,AmyJ.Bastian,2 and Jinger S. Gottschall

More information

Using GPOPS-II to optimize sum of squared torques of a double pendulum as a prosthesis leg. Abstract

Using GPOPS-II to optimize sum of squared torques of a double pendulum as a prosthesis leg. Abstract Using GPOPS-II to optimize sum of squared torques of a double pendulum as a prosthesis leg Abstract Milad Zarei MCE 593 Prosthesis Design & Control A two-dimensional, two links pendulum is developed to

More information

Stilt walking: How do we learn those first steps?

Stilt walking: How do we learn those first steps? Ergonomics Vol. 52, No. 9, September 2009, 1119 1127 Stilt walking: How do we learn those first steps? Sakineh B. Akram a * and James S. Frank b a Department of Kinesiology, University of Waterloo, 200

More information

Analysis of Skip Motion as a Recovery Strategy after an Induced Trip

Analysis of Skip Motion as a Recovery Strategy after an Induced Trip 2015 IEEE International Conference on Systems, Man, and Cybernetics Analysis of Skip Motion as a Recovery Strategy after an Induced Trip Kento Mitsuoka, Yasuhiro Akiyama, Yoji Yamada, and Shogo Okamoto

More information

GROUND REACTION FORCE DOMINANT VERSUS NON-DOMINANT SINGLE LEG STEP OFF

GROUND REACTION FORCE DOMINANT VERSUS NON-DOMINANT SINGLE LEG STEP OFF GROUND REACTION FORCE DOMINANT VERSUS NON-DOMINANT SINGLE LEG STEP OFF Sara Gharabaghli, Rebecca Krogstad, Sara Lynch, Sofia Saavedra, and Tamara Wright California State University, San Marcos, San Marcos,

More information

PURPOSE. METHODS Design

PURPOSE. METHODS Design 7 Murrary, M.P.; Sepic, S.B.; Gardner, G.M.; and Mollinger, L.A., "Gait patterns of above-knee amputees using constant-friction knee components," Bull Prosthet Res, 17(2):35-45, 1980. 8 Godfrey, C.M.;

More information

A New Approach to Modeling Vertical Stiffness in Heel-Toe Distance Runners

A New Approach to Modeling Vertical Stiffness in Heel-Toe Distance Runners Brigham Young University BYU ScholarsArchive All Faculty Publications 2003-12-01 A New Approach to Modeling Vertical Stiffness in Heel-Toe Distance Runners Iain Hunter iain_hunter@byu.edu Follow this and

More information

Modular Control of Limb Movements during Human Locomotion

Modular Control of Limb Movements during Human Locomotion The Journal of Neuroscience, October 10, 2007 27(41):11149 11161 11149 Behavioral/Systems/Cognitive Modular Control of Limb Movements during Human Locomotion Yuri P. Ivanenko, 1 Germana Cappellini, 1 Nadia

More information

Analysis of Foot Pressure Variation with Change in Stride Length

Analysis of Foot Pressure Variation with Change in Stride Length IOSR Journal of Dental and Medical Sciences (IOSR-JDMS) e-issn: 2279-853, p-issn: 2279-861.Volume 13, Issue 1 Ver. IV (Oct. 214), PP 46-51 Dr. Charudatta V. Shinde, M.S. MCh ( Orthopaedics ), Dr. Weijie

More information

Placing the trailing foot closer to an obstacle reduces flexion of the hip, knee, and ankle to increase the risk of tripping

Placing the trailing foot closer to an obstacle reduces flexion of the hip, knee, and ankle to increase the risk of tripping Journal of Biomechanics 31 (1998) 685 691 Placing the trailing foot closer to an obstacle reduces flexion of the hip, knee, and ankle to increase the risk of tripping Li-Shan Chou, Louis F. Draganich*

More information

Center of Mass Acceleration as a Surrogate for Force Production After Spinal Cord Injury Effects of Inclined Treadmill Walking

Center of Mass Acceleration as a Surrogate for Force Production After Spinal Cord Injury Effects of Inclined Treadmill Walking Center of Mass Acceleration as a Surrogate for Force Production After Spinal Cord Injury Effects of Inclined Treadmill Walking Mark G. Bowden, PhD, PT Research Health Scientist, Ralph H. Johnson VA Medical

More information

Influence of degraded visual acuity from light-scattering goggles on obstacle gait

Influence of degraded visual acuity from light-scattering goggles on obstacle gait Vol.3, No.2, 99-105 (2011) doi:10.4236/health.2011.32018 Health Influence of degraded visual acuity from light-scattering goggles on obstacle gait Shunsuke Yamaji 1*, Shinichi Demura 2, Hiroki Sugiura

More information

Spatial Methods for Road Course Measurement

Spatial Methods for Road Course Measurement Page 1 of 10 CurtinSearch Curtin Site Index Contact Details Links LASCAN Spatial Sciences WA Centre for Geodesy COURSE MEASUREMENT This page is a summary of results of some of the research we have recently

More information

Characteristics of ball impact on curve shot in soccer

Characteristics of ball impact on curve shot in soccer Available online at www.sciencedirect.com Procedia Engineering 60 (2013 ) 249 254 6 th Asia-Pacific Congress on Sports Technology (APCST) Characteristics of ball impact on curve shot in soccer Sungchan

More information

REPLACING REDUNDANT STABILOMETRY PARAMETERS WITH RATIO AND MAXIMUM DEVIATION PARAMETERS

REPLACING REDUNDANT STABILOMETRY PARAMETERS WITH RATIO AND MAXIMUM DEVIATION PARAMETERS Proceedings of the 12th IASTED International Conference Biomedical Engineering (BioMed 2016) February 15-16, 2016 Innsbruck, Austria REPLACING REDUNDANT STABILOMETRY PARAMETERS WITH RATIO AND MAXIMUM DEVIATION

More information

HPW Biomechanics

HPW Biomechanics HPW Biomechanics hpw@mail.com www.hpwbiomechanics.com ~ via e-mail ~ January 31, 213 To: Attn: From: Subject: I-Roc Debbie Chapman Janet S. Dufek, Ph.D. Research Scientist Additional Footwear Evaluation

More information

25 Lower-Limb Muscle Function in Human Running

25 Lower-Limb Muscle Function in Human Running 25 Lower-Limb Muscle Function in Human Running Anthony G. Schache, Tim W. Dorn, and Marcus G. Pandy * Abstract. This paper provides a brief summary of work completed to date in our research laboratory

More information

Sensory Gating for the Initiation of the Swing Phase in Different Directions of Human Infant Stepping

Sensory Gating for the Initiation of the Swing Phase in Different Directions of Human Infant Stepping The Journal of Neuroscience, July 1, 2002, 22(13):5734 5740 Sensory Gating for the Initiation of the Swing Phase in Different Directions of Human Infant Stepping Marco Y. C. Pang 1 and Jaynie F. Yang 1,2

More information

Assessments SIMPLY GAIT. Posture and Gait. Observing Posture and Gait. Postural Assessment. Postural Assessment 6/28/2016

Assessments SIMPLY GAIT. Posture and Gait. Observing Posture and Gait. Postural Assessment. Postural Assessment 6/28/2016 Assessments 2 SIMPLY GAIT Understanding movement Evaluations of factors that help therapist form professional judgments Include health, palpatory, range of motion, postural, and gait assessments Assessments

More information

Evaluation of Standing Stability and Reaching Postures on a Stepladder for Occupational Fall Prevention

Evaluation of Standing Stability and Reaching Postures on a Stepladder for Occupational Fall Prevention Evaluation of Standing Stability and Reaching Postures on a Stepladder for Occupational Fall Prevention Atsushi Sugama National Institute of Occupational Safety and Health, Japan (JNIOSH), Tokyo, Japan

More information

HOW DO WE ACCELERATE WHILE RUNNING? Daniel J. Schuster. April 2015

HOW DO WE ACCELERATE WHILE RUNNING? Daniel J. Schuster. April 2015 HOW DO WE ACCELERATE WHILE RUNNING? by Daniel J. Schuster April 2015 Director of Thesis: Dr. Paul DeVita Major Department: Kinesiology Running biomechanics are well established in terms of lower extremity

More information

Posterior-anterior body weight shift during stance period studied by measuring sole-floor reaction forces during healthy and hemiplegic human walking

Posterior-anterior body weight shift during stance period studied by measuring sole-floor reaction forces during healthy and hemiplegic human walking Neuroscience Letters 399 (2006) 141 146 Posterior-anterior body weight shift during stance period studied by measuring sole-floor reaction forces during healthy and hemiplegic human walking Nobuyoshi Kobayashi

More information

DIFFERENCE BETWEEN TAEKWONDO ROUNDHOUSE KICK EXECUTED BY THE FRONT AND BACK LEG - A BIOMECHANICAL STUDY

DIFFERENCE BETWEEN TAEKWONDO ROUNDHOUSE KICK EXECUTED BY THE FRONT AND BACK LEG - A BIOMECHANICAL STUDY 268 Isas 2000! Hong Kong DIFFERENCE BETWEEN TAEKWONDO ROUNDHOUSE KICK EXECUTED BY THE FRONT AND BACK LEG - A BIOMECHANICAL STUDY Pui-Wah Kong, Tze-Chung Luk and Youlian Hong The Chinese University of Hong

More information

Impact Points and Their Effect on Trajectory in Soccer

Impact Points and Their Effect on Trajectory in Soccer Proceedings Impact Points and Their Effect on Trajectory in Soccer Kaoru Kimachi 1, *, Sungchan Hong 2, Shuji Shimonagata 3 and Takeshi Asai 2 1 Doctoral Program of Coaching Science, University of Tsukuba,

More information

COMPARISON OF BIOMECHANICAL DATA OF A SPRINT CYCLIST IN THE VELODROME AND IN THE LABORATORY

COMPARISON OF BIOMECHANICAL DATA OF A SPRINT CYCLIST IN THE VELODROME AND IN THE LABORATORY COMPARISON OF BIOMECHANICAL DATA OF A SPRINT CYCLIST IN THE VELODROME AND IN THE LABORATORY Louise Burnie 1,2, Paul Barratt 3, Keith Davids 1, Paul Worsfold 2,4, Jon Wheat 1 Centre for Sports Engineering

More information

Barefoot Running. Ed Mulligan, PT, DPT, OCS, SCS, ATC. Clinical Orthopedic Rehabilitation Education

Barefoot Running. Ed Mulligan, PT, DPT, OCS, SCS, ATC. Clinical Orthopedic Rehabilitation Education Barefoot Running Ed Mulligan, PT, DPT, OCS, SCS, ATC Clinical Orthopedic Rehabilitation Education Not a new concept Nov 1958 1984 Olympics Interesting Trivia Abebe Bilika won the 1960 Olympic marathon

More information

Spatial invariance in anticipatory orienting behaviour during human navigation

Spatial invariance in anticipatory orienting behaviour during human navigation Neuroscience Letters 339 (2002) 243 247 www.elsevier.com/locate/neulet Spatial invariance in anticipatory orienting behaviour during human navigation Pascal Prévost a, *, Ivanenko Yuri b, Grasso Renato

More information

Normative data of postural sway by using sway meter among young healthy adults

Normative data of postural sway by using sway meter among young healthy adults Normative data of postural sway by using sway meter Original Research Article ISSN: 2394-0026 (P) Normative data of postural sway by using sway meter among young healthy adults Tejal C Nalawade 1*, Shyam

More information

Utilizing Virtual Reality to Understand Athletic Performance and Underlying Sensorimotor Processing

Utilizing Virtual Reality to Understand Athletic Performance and Underlying Sensorimotor Processing Proceedings Utilizing Virtual Reality to Understand Athletic Performance and Underlying Sensorimotor Processing Toshitaka Kimura *, Daiki Nasu and Makio Kashino Sports Brain Science Project, NTT Communication

More information

Efficient Variability: Linking Fractal Walking Patterns with Metabolic Energy Savings

Efficient Variability: Linking Fractal Walking Patterns with Metabolic Energy Savings Efficient Variability: Linking Fractal Walking Patterns with Metabolic Energy Savings Project Description Decades of research in biomechanics, neuroscience, and biology have produced two theories on why

More information

REPORT. A comparative study of the mechanical and biomechanical behaviour of natural turf and hybrid turf for the practise of sports

REPORT. A comparative study of the mechanical and biomechanical behaviour of natural turf and hybrid turf for the practise of sports REPORT A comparative study of the mechanical and biomechanical behaviour of natural turf and hybrid turf for the practise of sports Addressed to: PSF - PALAU TURF Date: May 2015 Table of Contents SHEET

More information

Biomechanical analysis of spiking skill in volleyball

Biomechanical analysis of spiking skill in volleyball 2017; 4(6): 15-19 P-ISSN: 2394-1685 E-ISSN: 2394-1693 Impact Factor (ISRA): 5.38 IJPESH 2017; 4(6): 15-19 2017 IJPESH www.kheljournal.com Received: 05-09-2017 Accepted: 06-10-2017 Harpreet Singh Assistant

More information

Supplementary Information

Supplementary Information Supplementary Information Novel robotic interface to evaluate, enable, and train locomotion and balance after neuromotor disorders Nadia Dominici, Urs Keller, Heike Vallery, Lucia Friedli, Rubia van den

More information

Foot Biomechanics Getting Back to the Base

Foot Biomechanics Getting Back to the Base Foot Biomechanics Getting Back to the Base Christopher J. Mehallo, D.O. Rothman Institute Orthopaedics Objectives Understand basic foot biomechanics during walking and running Understand common sports

More information

Ball impact dynamics of knuckling shot in soccer

Ball impact dynamics of knuckling shot in soccer Available online at www.sciencedirect.com Procedia Engineering 34 (2012 ) 200 205 9 th Conference of the International Sports Engineering Association (ISEA) Ball impact dynamics of knuckling shot in soccer

More information

Normal and Abnormal Gait

Normal and Abnormal Gait Normal and Abnormal Gait Adrielle Fry, MD EvergreenHealth, Division of Sport and Spine University of Washington Board Review Course March 6, 2017 What are we going to cover? Definitions and key concepts

More information

AN EXPERIMENTAL INVESTIGATION ON GOLF SHOE DESIGN USING FOOT- PRESSURE DISTRIBUTION DURING THE GOLF SWING

AN EXPERIMENTAL INVESTIGATION ON GOLF SHOE DESIGN USING FOOT- PRESSURE DISTRIBUTION DURING THE GOLF SWING Proceedings of the 4 th BSME-ASME International Conference on Thermal Engineering 27-29 December, 2008, Dhaka, Bangladesh AN EXPERIMENTAL INVESTIGATION ON GOLF SHOE DESIGN USING FOOT- PRESSURE DISTRIBUTION

More information

Gait & Posture 33 (2011) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Gait & Posture. journal homepage:

Gait & Posture 33 (2011) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Gait & Posture. journal homepage: Gait & Posture 33 (2011) 562 567 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Gait & Posture journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/gaitpost Braking and propulsive impulses increase with speed during accelerated

More information

Adaptation to Knee Flexion Torque Assistance in Double Support Phase

Adaptation to Knee Flexion Torque Assistance in Double Support Phase Adaptation to Knee Flexion Torque Assistance in Double Support Phase James S. Sulzer, Keith E. Gordon, T. George Hornby, Michael A. Peshkin and James L. Patton Abstract Studies have shown locomotor adaptation

More information

NEUROLOGICAL INSIGHTS FOR TEACHING GOLF TO TODAY S FITNESS CHALLENGED

NEUROLOGICAL INSIGHTS FOR TEACHING GOLF TO TODAY S FITNESS CHALLENGED NEUROLOGICAL INSIGHTS FOR TEACHING GOLF TO TODAY S FITNESS CHALLENGED John Milton, MD, PhD, FRCP(C) Director, Golf Neurology Clinic The University of Chicago Golf is fun. It is a game that all can play.

More information

Megan E. Krause, BSBSE, Young Hui Chang, Ph.D. Comparative Neuromechanics Laboratory. Georgia Institute of Technology

Megan E. Krause, BSBSE, Young Hui Chang, Ph.D. Comparative Neuromechanics Laboratory. Georgia Institute of Technology Megan E. Krause, BSBSE, Young Hui Chang, Ph.D. Comparative Neuromechanics Laboratory Sh School of Applied Physiology Georgia Institute of Technology 1 Variety of ankle constraints during locomotion: Fashion

More information

A Hare-Lynx Simulation Model

A Hare-Lynx Simulation Model 1 A Hare- Simulation Model What happens to the numbers of hares and lynx when the core of the system is like this? Hares O Balance? S H_Births Hares H_Fertility Area KillsPerHead Fertility Births Figure

More information

IMPORTANT: All features subject to change without notice.

IMPORTANT: All features subject to change without notice. 4.0.1 Table of Contents 1. Contents 4 2. Quick Start 4 3. Icon Key 5 4. Changing GaitSens View / Exiting GaitSens 5 5. Select User 6 6. Validate Setup 7 7. Therapist View 8 8. My Goals 9 9. Replay Session

More information

The Incremental Evolution of Gaits for Hexapod Robots

The Incremental Evolution of Gaits for Hexapod Robots The Incremental Evolution of Gaits for Hexapod Robots Abstract Gait control programs for hexapod robots are learned by incremental evolution. The first increment is used to learn the activations required

More information

The Journal of Physiology Neuroscience

The Journal of Physiology Neuroscience J Physiol 595.1 (217) pp 341 361 341 The Journal of Physiology Neuroscience Left right coordination from simple to extreme conditions during split-belt locomotion in the chronic spinal adult cat Alain

More information

Mutual and asynchronous anticipation and action in sports as globally competitive

Mutual and asynchronous anticipation and action in sports as globally competitive 1 Supplementary Materials Mutual and asynchronous anticipation and action in sports as globally competitive and locally coordinative dynamics Keisuke Fujii, Tadao Isaka, Motoki Kouzaki and Yuji Yamamoto.

More information

Inter and intra-limb processes of gait control.

Inter and intra-limb processes of gait control. Inter and intra-limb processes of gait control. Simon B. Taylor, Rezaul K. Begg and Russell J. Best Biomechanics Unit, Centre for Rehabilitation, Exercise and Sport Science. Victoria University, Melbourne,

More information

Gait Analysis by High School Students

Gait Analysis by High School Students Gait Analysis by High School Students André Heck 1 and Caroline van Dongen 2 1 AMSTEL Institute, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands, heck@science.uva.nl 2 St. Antoniuscollege, Gouda, The

More information

CHAPTER IV FINITE ELEMENT ANALYSIS OF THE KNEE JOINT WITHOUT A MEDICAL IMPLANT

CHAPTER IV FINITE ELEMENT ANALYSIS OF THE KNEE JOINT WITHOUT A MEDICAL IMPLANT 39 CHAPTER IV FINITE ELEMENT ANALYSIS OF THE KNEE JOINT WITHOUT A MEDICAL IMPLANT 4.1 Modeling in Biomechanics The human body, apart of all its other functions is a mechanical mechanism and a structure,

More information

Posture influences ground reaction force: implications for crouch gait

Posture influences ground reaction force: implications for crouch gait University of Tennessee, Knoxville From the SelectedWorks of Jeffrey A. Reinbolt July 14, 2010 Posture influences ground reaction force: implications for crouch gait H. X. Hoang Jeffrey A. Reinbolt, University

More information