This is the author s version of a work that was submitted/accepted for publication in the following source:

Similar documents
This is the author s version of a work that was submitted/accepted for publication in the following source:

Driver Response to Phase Termination at Signalized Intersections at Signalized Intersections: Are Driving Simulator Results Valid?

MRI-2: Integrated Simulation and Safety

The Effects of Dual-Task Interference and Response Strategy on Stop or Go Decisions to Yellow Light Changes

Real Time Bicycle Simulation Study of Bicyclists Behaviors and their Implication on Safety

MTCF. Michigan Traffic Crash Facts FACT SHEETS

Factors Leading to Older Drivers Intersection Crashes

MTCF. Michigan Traffic Crash Facts FACT SHEETS

Every time a driver is distracted,

Evaluating Road Departure Crashes Using Naturalistic Driving Study Data

2014 QUICK FACTS ILLINOIS CRASH INFORMATION. Illinois Emergency Medical Services for Children February 2016 Edition

2012 QUICK FACTS ILLINOIS CRASH INFORMATION. Illinois Emergency Medical Services for Children September 2014 Edition

Crashes of older Australian riders

A Conceptual Approach for Using the UCF Driving Simulator as a Test Bed for High Risk Locations

Pedestrian and Driver Distraction: Overview & NHTSA Prevalence and Risk Study. Heidi Coleman, NHTSA Bob Scopatz, VHB

Crosswalk event overview (Saint Paul) How to organize a crosswalk event. 1. Choose a date, time and location

EMS professionals face many risks exposure to infectious diseases, violence, hazardous scenes, and oncoming traffic, to name a few.

MRI-2: Integrated Simulation and Safety

Using SHRP 2 s NDS Video Data to Evaluate the Impact of Offset Left-Turn Lanes on Gap Acceptance Behavior Karin M. Bauer & Jessica M.

Driving Simulators for Evaluation of Novel Traffic-Control Devices

Driver Behavior at Highway-Rail Grade Crossings With Passive Traffic Controls

STATIC AND DYNAMIC EVALUATION OF THE DRIVER SPEED PERCEPTION AND SELECTION PROCESS

TRAINER NOTES FOR LESSON PLAN School Bus Drivers Inservice TITLE OF LESSON: LOADING & UNLOADING STUDENTS SAFELY

WATCH OUT FOR PEDESTRIANS!

Human Factors for Limited-Ability Autonomous Driving Systems

Driver Education Ch. 4: Safe Driving Rules & Regulations. Ms. Marx

Figure 1. Indiana fatal collisions by young driver involvement,

Design Considerations for Complete Streets. The Kiewit Center for Infrastructure and Transportation

Double Pair Comparisons PART III AGE & GENDER. Age and Crash Risk. Subject & control groups Relative risk or rate. Relative Accident Involvement Ratio

Safety Assessment of Installing Traffic Signals at High-Speed Expressway Intersections

Important note To cite this publication, please use the final published version (if applicable). Please check the document version above.

ANALYSIS OF RURAL CURVE NEGOTIATION USING NATURALISTIC DRIVING DATA Nicole Oneyear and Shauna Hallmark

Cyclists and red lights a study of behaviour of commuter cyclists in Melbourne

Covert Texting During Simulated Driving Maneuvers Effects of Head-Up versus Head-Down Posture

USER GUIDE FOR R1-6 GATEWAY TREATMENT

RAMP CROSSWALK TREATMENT FOR SAN DIEGO AIRPORT, TERMINAL ONE

USER GUIDE FOR R1-6 GATEWAY TREATMENT

Final Report. Research. Investigating the Effects of Traffic Calming Strategies on Driver Behavior

Officer Safety on our Roadways

WORKING LESSON PLAN School Bus Driver In-Service

People killed and injured per million hours spent travelling, Motorcyclist Cyclist Driver Car / van passenger

Risk Homeostasis Theory in Traffic Safety

EVALUATING THE IMPACT OF TWO ALLOWABLE PERMISSIVE LEFT-TURN INDICATIONS

Alberta. Traffic Collision Statistics. Office of Traffic Safety Transportation Services Division May 2017

THE EPIDEMIOLOGY OF TRAUMATIC BRAIN INJURIES IN NEW YORK STATE

GUIDELINES FOR EMERGENCY TRAFFIC CONTROL

Tampa Bay. Pedestrian Safety Skills

Driver Yielding at Midblock Crossings Based on Roadway, Traffic, and Crosswalk Characteristics

2013 Traffic Safety Report

To lane-split or not lane-split: Can the UC Berkeley Study help me decide?

TO: Director, National Institute for Occupational Safety and Health. SUBJECT: City Street Worker Was Struck and Killed by a Speeding Car

OBSERVATION OF GAP ACCEPTANCE DURING INTERSECTION APPROACH

the Ministry of Transport is attributed as the source of the material

BTW Lesson 5. Traffic Flow

Mobileye Shield+ Collision Avoidance System Pilot Program

Maine Highway Safety Facts 2016

An Intervention to Reduce Distracted Walking

officer Traffic Safety National Law Enforcement Officers Memorial Fund September 2012

Toward Zero Deaths: Proactive Steps for Your Community

Reconstructing a rollover includes examining physical evidence on the roadway and the vehicle. Here s a detailed roadmap to examining that evidence

CHAPTER 2 LITERATURE REVIEW

2012 TOWN OF CASTLE ROCK MOTOR VEHICLE ACCIDENT FACTS PREPARED BY THE PUBLIC WORKS DEPARTMENT

The Case for Roundabouts:

DRIVER S LICENCE HANDBOOK

Preventing Rear End Collisions. OSACH Safety Group Meeting April 16, 2009

Mayor s Pedestrian Advisory Council. Wednesday, February 15

SPEED PROFILES APPROACHING A TRAFFIC SIGNAL

Topsham, ME. September 20, A Joint Project of the Maine Department of Transportation and the Bicycle Coalition of Maine

Public Opinion, Traffic Performance, the Environment, and Safety After Construction of Double-Lane Roundabouts

Bicycles and Pedestrians on ISU Campus. Indiana State University Police

2015 Victorian Road Trauma. Analysis of Fatalities and Serious Injuries. Updated 5 May Page 1 of 28. Commercial in Confidence

Multilane Roundabouts

Subject: Use of Pull-off Areas in Work Zones Page: 1 of 13. Brief Description: Guidance for the use and placement of pull-off area in work zones.

A Traffic Operations Method for Assessing Automobile and Bicycle Shared Roadways

ENGINEER S PRELIMINARY REPORT. for the #######-###### COLLISION

Crash Facts Scenario 1 Pedestrian Accident - Solution - May 2006

BLACK SPOTS MANAGEMENT - SLOVENIAN EXPERIENCE

DOI /HORIZONS.B P23 UDC : (497.11) PEDESTRIAN CROSSING BEHAVIOUR AT UNSIGNALIZED CROSSINGS 1

Department of Transportation

BICYCLING ON ROADWAYS

Why Zone In on Speed Reduction?

PEDESTRIAN GAP ACCEPTANCE FOR MID-BLOCK STREET CROSSING

Emergency Roadside Technician Dies When Struck by a Single-Unit Truck on an Interstate Shoulder. Incident Number: 05KY001

Bangor, ME. August 9, A Joint Project of the Maine Department of Transportation and the Bicycle Coalition of Maine

How would you like it if people drove their cars thru your office?

SAFETY OF OUR STREETS

Fatal School Bus Collision North of London Ontario

LOS ANGELES COUNTY FIRE DEPARMENT APPARATUS OPERATOR 1C TILLERED EMERGENCY VEHICLE OPERATIONS

Use of Throw Distances of Pedestrians and Bicyclists as Part of a Scientific Accident Reconstruction Method 1

17th Annual California Senior Injury Prevention Educational Forum

ROAD SAFETY AWARENESS INDEX & ROAD USER BEHAVIOR- A CASE STUDY AT KAZHAKKOOTTAM

Geometric Categories as Intersection Safety Evaluation Tools

Florida Class E Knowledge Exam Road Rules Practice Questions

Advance Yield Markings Reduce Motor Vehicle/Pedestrian. Conflicts at Multilane Crosswalks with an Uncontrolled Approach.

Development of Analytical Tools to Evaluate Road Departure Crashes Using Naturalistic Driving Study Data

Arlington County Police Department Law Enforcement Guide. Enforcing Bicycle Traffic Laws

SHRP 2. Strategic Highway Research Program. Accelerating solutions for highway safety and performance. Charles Fay, Sr.

Bicycle Collision Analysis Using a Vehicle Driving Simulator MOVIC-T4

Deaths/injuries in motor vehicle crashes per million hours spent travelling, July 2007 June 2011 (All ages) Mode of travel

Draft Report: Traffic Crashes in the Denver Region

Transcription:

This is the author s version of a work that was submitted/accepted for publication in the following source: Haque, Md. Mazharul, Washington, Simon, Ohlhauser, Amanda, & Boyle, Linda (2012) Effects of Mobile Phone Distraction at the Onset of Amber Light: Analysis of Driving Simulator Data. In QUT Transport Policy, Planning and Engineering Symposium, 15 Jun 2012, Brisbane. (Unpublished) This file was downloaded from: https://eprints.qut.edu.au/51261/ c Copyright 2012 the authors. Notice: Changes introduced as a result of publishing processes such as copy-editing and formatting may not be reflected in this document. For a definitive version of this work, please refer to the published source:

Effects of Mobile Phone Distraction at the Onset of Amber Light: Analysis of Driving Simulator Data Dr Md. Mazharul Haque, Prof. Simon Washington, Amanda aven & Prof Linda Boyle MD. MAZHAUL HAQUE, PhD Civil Engineering and the Built Environment & Centre for Accident esearch and oad Safety - Queensland (CAS-Q) Queensland University of Technology (QUT) Queensland University of Technology Background AGENDA Experimental Setup Statistical Modeling esults & Discussions Conclusion 1

BACKGOUND U.S. roadways Driving distraction claimed about 5,474 fatalities and 448,000 injuries in 2009. Cell phone distraction alone caused about 995 people to be killed and 24,000 to be injured. Australian roadways About 2,400 distraction related incidents were reported in NSW (2002) mostly by young drivers using cell phone. In 2010, ACT police issued 2,323 infringement notices and 445 caution notices to drivers using cell phone. Media Watch BACKGOUND The Daily Telegraph June 01, 2012 The Courier Mail November 11, 2011 2

BACKGOUND esearch on Cell Phone Distraction Speed Control Vehicle Control Lane Departures Braking Performances eaction Time Behavior at Signalized Intersections Stop/Go Decision at Amber Light An improper decision i at amber light may cause red light running or abrupt stop. Amber lights were problematic for both younger and older drivers (Koneci et al., 1976; Edwards et al., 2003). BACKGOUND Stop/Go Decision at Amber Light Novice drivers were more likely to run through an amber light (Senserrick et al., 2007). Higher time to stop line decreased the likelihood of running through amber light (Caird et al., 2007). Cell phone distraction increased the likelihood of failure to detect traffic signals (Strayer and Johnston, 2001) esearch Question In general, phone conversation increases perception response time to critical driving events (e.g., Consiglio et al., 2003). How do cell phone distracted drivers response at the onset of amber light? 3

BACKGOUND OBJECTIVE To examine the decisions of distracted drivers at the onset of amber light. SCOPE A group of distracted drivers were exposed on a series of signalized intersections using the National Advanced Driving Simulator of IOWA. EXPEIMENTAL SETUP Driving Simulator (NADS 1) 360 0 driver view in a 24 feet dome Dome can rotate up to 330 0 in its vertical axis 13 degree of freedom motion base Two belts driven beams to provide lateral and longitudinal accelerations Data are recorded at rates up to 240 Hz 4

Participants EXPEIMENTAL SETUP Four age groups: Novice, 16-17 years old; Males = 11, Females = 0 Licensed between 4 and 8 weeks Younger, 18-25 years old; Males = 9, Females = 8 Middle, 30-45 years old; Males = 9, Females = 8 Older, 50-60 years old; Males = 8, Females = 5 Procedure Briefing about how to use the simulator and cell phone apparatus A familiarization drive before the experimental drive. Procedure EXPEIMENTAL SETUP Six signalized intersections on the simulated driving route 2 in the baseline condition (with no device) 4 in the handheld phone condition Traffic lights changed from green to amber when the driven car was 3.0 seconds from the stop line (3 intersections) 3.75 seconds from the stop line (3 intersections) Cell Phone Conversation Cognitive type of conversation task Drivers needed to determine whether a sentence made sense or not. 5

Variables STATISTICAL MODELING Stop/Go decision (Binary response) Phone condition (baseline, handheld) Driver s Age group (novice, younger, middle, older) Gender (male, female) Time to stop line (TSL): varies between 2.46 and 3.85 seconds Statistical Model Driver s decision = f (age, gender, TSL, phone condition) epeated measures Logistic egression model with a combination of a Decision Tree analysis. Decision Tree ESULTS Interaction Variable1: A novice driver without phone conversation and distance from the stop line at yellow light 188.5 ft 6

Logistic egression ESULTS (CNTD.) Time to Stop Line (TSL) 1 sec increase in TSL was associated with 9.7 fold increase in stopping at amber light. Handheld (HH) Phone Condition Overall the phone conversation decreased the likelihood of proceeding through amber light. TSL x HH 1 sec increase in TSL was associated with 10.6 times increase in running through an amber light for distracted drivers. Driver s Age The middle aged group was the least likely to run an amber light, followed by older drivers, and younger drivers. Logistic egression ESULTS (CNTD.) Age x TSL The likelihood of running through amber light increased with time to stop line for younger and older drivers Higher Order Interaction Variables A novice driver was more likely to run through amber when the distance was 188.4 ft. A young driver was about 11 times more likely to run the amber light when the speed was > 44.9 mph. An older male driver was more likely to run through the amber light when the speed was > 46 mph. TSL: Time to Stop Line 7

Logistic egression ESULTS (CNTD.) Gender Female drivers were more likely to run through an amber light. Age x HH Novice and young drivers were more likely to run the amber light when distracted by cell phone conversation. Age x HH x TSL As TSL increases, distracted novice and young drivers were less likely to proceed through the amber light. TSL : Time to Stop Line HH : Handheld Phone DISCUSSION 1 Non Distracted eference Group Distracted eference Group Non Distracted Male Novice Distracted Male Novice Non Distracted Male Younger Distracted Male Younger Non Distracted Male Older Distracted Male Older Non Distracted Female Younger Distracted Female Younger Non Distracted Female Older Distracted Female Older nning through an amber lig ght Probability of ru 0.8 0.6 0.4 0.2 0 0.5 1 1.5 2 2.5 3 3.5 4 4.5 5 Time to the stop line (sec) 8

1 DISCUSSION (CNTD.) Non Distracted eference Group Non Distracted Male Novice Distracted eference Group Distracted Male Novice ty of running through an amber light Probabilit 0.8 0.6 0.4 0.2 70% 30% 0 0.5 1 1.5 2 2.5 3 3.5 4 4.5 5 Time to the stop line (sec) ity of running through an amber light Probabili 1 0.8 0.6 0.4 0.2 48% DISCUSSION (CNTD.) Non Distracted Male Younger Non Distracted Female Younger Distracted Male Younger Distracted Female Younger Avg. eduction 30% 62% 40% 0 0.5 1 1.5 2 2.5 3 3.5 4 4.5 5 Time to the stop line (sec) 10% 9

DISCUSSION (CNTD.) lity of running through an amber light 1 0.8 0.6 0.4 0.2 Distracted Male Older Distracted Female Older Probabi 0 0.5 1 1.5 2 2.5 3 3.5 4 4.5 5 Time to the stop line (sec) CONCLUSION Drivers responses to distraction and running an amber signal vary considerably across age and gender. Young and novice drivers are more likely to run through an amber light while distracted by cell phone conversations. Overall, drivers tend to compensate for the increased risk of phone conversation by running the amber light less often. isk compensation is also exhibited across most tested groups. Whether the risk compensation is sufficient to offset crash risks we don tknow; but on road situation ti suggest it is not. The behavior of older drivers and gender differences require further investigations. 10