Detecting Transfer of Training Through Simulator Scenario Design: A Novice Driver Training Study

Similar documents
STATIC AND DYNAMIC EVALUATION OF THE DRIVER SPEED PERCEPTION AND SELECTION PROCESS

Access Location, Spacing, Turn Lanes, and Medians

EVALUATING THE IMPACT OF TWO ALLOWABLE PERMISSIVE LEFT-TURN INDICATIONS

The Effects of Dual-Task Interference and Response Strategy on Stop or Go Decisions to Yellow Light Changes

CROSSING GUARD PLACEMENT CONSIDERATIONS AND GAP ASSESSMENT

A Traffic Operations Method for Assessing Automobile and Bicycle Shared Roadways

MRI-2: Integrated Simulation and Safety

Driver Response to Phase Termination at Signalized Intersections at Signalized Intersections: Are Driving Simulator Results Valid?

Montana Teen Driver Education and Training. Module 3.3. Mixing with Traffic. Montana Teen Driver Curriculum

Ohio Share the Road Driver s s Education Unit

BTW Lesson 5. Traffic Flow

Formative Evaluation of Engineering Designs Using Driver Performance in a Immersive Driving Simulator

Road Safety Assessment

HUMAN (DRIVER) ERRORS

Identify the letter of the choice that best completes the statement or answers the question.

Alberta. Traffic Collision Statistics. Office of Traffic Safety Transportation Services Division May 2017

Identify the letter of the choice that best completes the statement or answers the question.\

DRIVING ON EXPRESSWAYS/ INTERSTATE CHAPTER 11

Bicycle - Motor Vehicle Collisions on Controlled Access Highways in Arizona

The Effect of External Distractions on Novice and Experienced Drivers' Anticipation of Hazards and Vehicle Control

Statistical Assessment of the Glare Issue - Human and Natural Elements

WORKING LESSON PLAN School Bus Driver In-Service

Every time a driver is distracted,

Pedestrian and Driver Distraction: Overview & NHTSA Prevalence and Risk Study. Heidi Coleman, NHTSA Bob Scopatz, VHB

DOT HS September Crash Factors in Intersection-Related Crashes: An On-Scene Perspective

133 rd Street and 132 nd /Hemlock Street 132 nd Street and Foster Street MINI ROUNDABOUTS. Overland Park, Kansas

Module 5: Navigating Roadways

Access Management in the Vicinity of Intersections

Car Following by Optical Parameters

Evaluation of Pedestrian and Cyclist Warning Systems for Trucks

2014 QUICK FACTS ILLINOIS CRASH INFORMATION. Illinois Emergency Medical Services for Children February 2016 Edition

2012 QUICK FACTS ILLINOIS CRASH INFORMATION. Illinois Emergency Medical Services for Children September 2014 Edition

MEMORANDUM. Charlotte Fleetwood, Transportation Planner

Chapter 4 Traffic Analysis

City of Vallejo Traffic Calming Toolbox

An Application of Signal Detection Theory for Understanding Driver Behavior at Highway-Rail Grade Crossings

HEADWAY AND SAFETY ANALYSIS OF SPEED LAW ENFORCEMENT TECHNIQUES IN HIGHWAY WORK ZONES

This is the author s version of a work that was submitted/accepted for publication in the following source:

Existing Conditions. Date: April 16 th, Dan Holderness; Coralville City Engineer Scott Larson; Coralville Assistant City Engineer

Driver Training School Instructor Curriculum Requirements for Student Learning & Performance Goals

Driver compliance with, and understanding of, level crossing controls

Texas Driver Education Classroom and In-car Instruction Model Curriculum

GUIDELINES FOR USING DECISION SIGHT DISTANCE AT IGNALIZED INTERSECTIONS NEAR VERTICAL CURVES

Driving with Para-Central Visual Field Loss: Pilot Study

Driver Behavior at Highway-Rail Grade Crossings With Passive Traffic Controls

Unit Six: Driving Faster with More Risk URBAN, SUBURBAN, AND RURAL DRIVING

Visual Attention Failures during Turns at Intersections: An On-road Study *

Why do you think the chances of a collision are greater at an intersection than at any other point on the roadway?

Saturation Flow Rate, Start-Up Lost Time, and Capacity for Bicycles at Signalized Intersections


Covert Texting During Simulated Driving Maneuvers Effects of Head-Up versus Head-Down Posture

Watching Out for Us! Skills for Safe Walking. Presenter Guide

Geometric Categories as Intersection Safety Evaluation Tools

Unit Five : Driving In Neighborhoods

The automobile is a complex system in which humans play an important role. Driving is

The Effect of Pavement Marking on Speed. Reduction in Exclusive Motorcycle Lane. in Malaysia

Cycling and risk. Cycle facilities and risk management

RAMP CROSSWALK TREATMENT FOR SAN DIEGO AIRPORT, TERMINAL ONE

Safety Assessment of Installing Traffic Signals at High-Speed Expressway Intersections

Multiple Threat Crashes

A Conceptual Approach for Using the UCF Driving Simulator as a Test Bed for High Risk Locations

Bicycle-Specific Traffic Control Is it "Bicycle-Friendly"?

EMS professionals face many risks exposure to infectious diseases, violence, hazardous scenes, and oncoming traffic, to name a few.

VT15 / Allen Martin Drive Intersection Scoping Study

Exploring Older Driver Lateral Head Rotations at Intersections Using Naturalistic Driving Data

Economic and Social Council

Safety and Design Alternatives for Two-Way Stop-Controlled Expressway Intersections

Bicycle and Pedestrian Level of Service Evaluation

Chapter 2 Defensive Driving

The Walkability Indicator. The Walkability Indicator: A Case Study of the City of Boulder, CO. College of Architecture and Planning

ITE Northeastern District Meeting

Statement before the Maryland Senate Committee on Judicial Proceedings on Senate Bill 277. Research on Automated Speed Enforcement. Stephen L.

The Animated Eyes Symbol as Part of the WALK Signal: An Examination of the Generality of its Effectiveness Across a Variety of

At each type of conflict location, the risk is affected by certain parameters:

Advance Yield Markings Reduce Motor Vehicle/Pedestrian. Conflicts at Multilane Crosswalks with an Uncontrolled Approach.

Automated Proactive Road Safety Analysis

An application of the Safe System Approach to a set of self-reported cycling crashes

People killed and injured per million hours spent travelling, Motorcyclist Cyclist Driver Car / van passenger

On-Road Evaluation of Driver Capability: A Medical Record Review of the Adaptive Driving Program

ITS Animated LED Signals. Alert Drivers to Pedestrian Threats

US Hwy. 64/264 Pedestrian Crossing at the Little Bridge Alternatives Analysis Public Meeting

Effectiveness of Visual Collision Warning Alerts on Young Drivers Latent Hazard Anticipation

THE EFFECTIVENESS OF LOW-COST TRAFFIC CALMING APPLICATIONS APPROPRIATE FOR MAIN STREETS THROUGH RURAL COMMUNITIES

Emergency Rides. Driving Simulators Research Development Production. 1. General. Rev

Pedestrian Scenario Design and Performance Assessment in Driving Simulations

CITY OF WEST KELOWNA COUNCIL POLICY MANUAL

We will also cover topics including sharing the road with: Emergency vehicles; Pedestrians; and Animals.

HSIS. Association of Selected Intersection Factors With Red-Light-Running Crashes. State Databases Used SUMMARY REPORT

Preventing Rear End Collisions. OSACH Safety Group Meeting April 16, 2009

Cyclists gaze behavior in urban space: an eye-tracking experiment on bicycle facilities.

Designing for Pedestrian Safety

TOO CLOSE FOR COMFORT: EVALUATING A REWARD-BASED APPROACH TO INCREASE DRIVERS HEADWAY

An evaluation of intersection characteristics associated with crashes at intersections in Melbourne CBD. Jim Scully, Brian Fildes 1

Visual attention and the transition from novice to advanced driver

LECTUR 10 CHARACTERISTICS OF THE DRIVER, THE PEDESTRIAN, THE VEHICLE AND THE ROAD. One problem that faces traffic and transportation engineers is:

An intelligent approach that works for all Brake Fleet Safety Conference 2016 Nick O Donnell, Assistant Director Strategic Transport, Ealing Council

AGE, SKILL, AND HAZARD PERCEPTION IN DRIVING

Pedestrian injuries in San Francisco: distribution, causes, and solutions

Why Zone In on Speed Reduction?

Department of Transportation

Transcription:

University of Iowa Iowa Research Online Driving Assessment Conference 2011 Driving Assessment Conference Jun 28th, 12:00 AM Detecting Transfer of Training Through Simulator Scenario Design: A Novice Driver Training Study Wade Allen Systems Technology, Inc., Hawthorne, CA George Park Systems Technology, Inc., Hawthorne, CA Scott Terrace Systems Technology, Inc., Hawthorne, CA John Grant Systems Technology, Inc., Hawthorne, CA Follow this and additional works at: http://ir.uiowa.edu/drivingassessment Allen, Wade; Park, George; Terrace, Scott; and Grant, John. Detecting Transfer of Training Through Simulator Scenario Design: A Novice Driver Training Study. In: Proceedings of the Sixth International Driving Symposium on Human Factors in Driver Assessment, Training and Vehicle Design, June 27-30, 2011, Olympic Valley Lake Tahoe, California. Iowa City, IA: Public Policy Center, University of Iowa, 2011: 203-210. https://doi.org/10.17077/drivingassessment.1398 This Event is brought to you for free and open access by the Public Policy Center at Iowa Research Online. It has been accepted for inclusion in Driving Assessment Conference by an authorized administrator of Iowa Research Online. For more information, please contact lib-ir@uiowa.edu.

DETECTING TRANSFER OF TRAINING THROUGH SIMULATOR SCENARIO DESIGN: A NOVICE DRIVER TRAINING STUDY Wade Allen, George Park, Scott Terrace, & John Grant Systems Technology, Inc. Hawthorne, California, USA Email: rwallen@systemstech.com Summary: Novice drivers in comparison to experienced drivers perform poorly due to incomplete mental models of roadway hazards. This paper describes the driving simulator scenario design methods used in a novice driver training study to detect a possible transfer of training for hazard perception. Applied in a high school driver education classroom, the data of trained versus un-trained drivers is presented for pre/post-test driving scenarios, N = 67. Results showed that while general simulator control performance between the trained and un-trained groups was similar, the trained group performed better at hazard events and exhibited fewer speeding behaviors at the post-test. Specific hazard encounters indicated that simulator training may have had an effect on performance even when the training group was not trained on the specific situation. Arguments for training transfer in hazard perception are presented. INTRODUCTION Previous studies have indicated that novice drivers visually scan the roadway environment more narrowly and miss more hazardous targets in comparison to experienced drivers (Mourant & Rockwell, 1972; Pradhan, Pollatsek, Knodler, & Fisher, 2009; Underwood, 2007). As shown in studies that remove the driving component (Underwood, 2007), novice driver inhibitions in visual scanning are not necessarily due to high vehicle control demands, but a lack of situational awareness or an incomplete mental model of roadway dangers. Analyses of driver crash data support this notion of inattention to potentially hazardous driving situations as a major contributor to the elevated crash risk of novice drivers (McKnight & McKnight, 2003). To address this problem, past studies have introduced novice driver training programs to improve hazard perception using carefully designed birds-eye-view images of the roadway (Pradhan, et al., 2009), real-time traffic videos (Underwood, 2007), and driving simulation scenarios requiring collision avoidance (Allen et al., 2005). The purpose of this paper is to describe the driving simulator scenario design methods used in a novice driver training study to detect a transfer of training for hazard perception in the driving environment. Presented is the data collected during a single semester at a high school driver education classroom. Two classroom periods received simulation training (trained group) while two classrooms periods did not (control group). All groups received a pre- and post-test drive on the simulator at the beginning and end of the semester. Transfer of training is traditionally detected using a different problem context, modality or testing platform, e.g., a driver is trained on a simulator, followed by an on-road vehicle test or monitoring of real world crash rates. However, for some studies this is not always feasible. As 203

such, it was hypothesized that if pre- and post-test drive scenarios could be designed such that driving hazards in both tests are matched in spatial and temporal elements, but different in driving context using simulation elements (e.g., building models, ground textures, roadway type, background scenery, etc ), priming cues to previously exposed hazard events could be minimized and evidence for training transfer could be detected. Furthermore, additional support may be found with the inclusion of a novel collision event in the post-test. METHODS Participants A total of 67 driver education students (38 female, 29 male; ages 14 17 yrs.) with signed parental consent participated in the study for a single semester as part of general curriculum course work. All participants were from a single public high school located in Manhattan, Kansas and were instructed by the same driver education teacher. Driving Simulator As shown in Figure 1, a single-monitor (19 ), fixed-based, desktop driving simulator was used. Driver controls were Logitech PC gaming units. The simulation laboratory consisted of five simulator stations and was located adjacent to the driver education classroom. Pre & Post Test Scenarios Pre/post test drives were designed to appear as different as possible using simulation elements (e.g., far horizon, building models, and ground textures), while still retaining critical testing elements. For example, both pre/post test drives were designed with a matched and counterbalanced number of intersections (9), signal light presentations (2 green, 1 amber), 4-way stop signs (4), speed limit zones (20, 25, 45, and 40-70 mph), traffic environments (school, residential, urban, and rural), pedestrian collision events (5), and vehicle collision events (4). Collision events were presented with the same spatial (e.g., originating from left or right) and temporal timing (i.e., time-to-collision). Furthermore, no auditory or visual feedback (e.g., crash noises or vehicle speed/position resets) was provided to drivers for poor performance (e.g., vehicle/ped collisions, missed stop signs) during both test drives. This was intended to reduce artificial increases in driver vigilance due to crash/violation alerts and maintain the integrity of the driver s behavior (good or bad) during testing. Performance feedback in terms of a run summary with listed error counts was only provided after the test drives and during the training drives for the training group students. Specific hazard events were also designed for the pre/post test drives to detect possible group differences and training effects. Driver training was specifically provided for certain events, while for other events no training was given. Obscured Stop Sign Event. This event involved a 4-way intersection stop with the driver s stop sign obscured by a large vehicle. The situation awareness in this event involves learning the various details associated with a four-way stop (cross street and markings) so that even though 204

the stop sign is obscured, the driver still has sufficient cues to indicate a stop is required. As shown in Figure 2, the pre-assessment drive used a bus in a low speed, residential school zone to obscure the sign whereas the post-test drive used a large construction truck in a mid speed, rural town area. While training drives provided practice with four-way stop signs, at no time were they obscured by another object. Only in the pre and post test drives did this particular event appear. Pre-Test (school zone area) (rural town area) Figure 2. Obscured Stop Sign Event (driver & aerial view) for pre- & post-test drives Head-On Collision Event. This event involved an oncoming vehicle passing a slower larger vehicle by entering the driver s lane. The appropriate driver reaction is to reduce speed and move to the shoulder once collision appears eminent. Specific training for this event was given in the rural training drives using the same driving contexts as in the pre-test drive. As shown in Figure 3, the post-test drive kept a high speed rural environment but used a mountain driving scene. Pre-Test (prairie) Post Test (mountain) Figure 3. Head On Collision Event (driver & aerial view) for pre- & post-test drives Vehicle Back Out Event. This event involved a perpendicularly parked vehicle backing out into the driver s path from the right, requiring a sudden stop to avoid a collision. Specific training was provided for this event in the residential training drives using the same low speed, residential environment as in the pre-test. As shown in Figure 4, the post-test drive used in contrast a midspeed, rural town environment. Pre-Test (residential) Post Test (rural town) Figure 4. Vehicle Back Out Event (driver & aerial view) for pre- & post-test drives Vehicle Backout + Head-On Event. This event was intended to provide a novel driving situation, where drivers had to put themselves into another driver s point of view. As shown in Figure 5, a white SUV on the left, backed out onto the approaching traffic lane while obscured by a larger 205

yellow truck to an approaching red car. The approaching red car must then make an emergency maneuver into the driver s lane to avoid the collision. This maneuver in turn creates a head-on collision event with the driver. As the white SUV is backing out, the driver should foresee this possible outcome with the approaching red vehicle by braking early enough to allow the approaching red car s swerving behavior. While specific training was provided for vehicle backouts and head-on collisions independently, this combination event was only provided in the post-test drive and early in the scenario to reduce any event testing confounds. Testing Procedure & Training Curriculum A computer-based instruction program was developed to login, track, and administer the training curriculum. As displayed in Table 1, the testing and training curriculum comprised of eight sessions (approx. 10-20 min each) with first and last sessions used for orientation and pre/post test drives. On average, students completed one session a week with groups of students entering the simulator lab during normal driver education class times. Driving lessons were designed based on speed environments (low speed residential, mid speed urban, and high speed rural) and designed similar in appearance to the pre-test drive. Educational materials (slides and videos) of hazard situations and risky driving practices were presented at the start of each of the three lessons, followed by a short multiple choice quiz to check knowledge retention. Within each lesson, the four training drives progressed with increasing difficulty in terms of pedestrians, traffic flow, and hazardous events. Collision events (e.g., pedestrian crossings, vehicle pullouts) were counterbalanced to increase difficulty. Additional training scenario details have been described elsewhere (Terrace, Park & Allen, 2010). While training group students were given all sessions, control group students were divided into two additional groups: 1) no educational materials (sessions 1 & 8 only) and 2) educational materials for eight sessions. The latter group received the same slideshow materials as the simulator trained group but with additional materials in order to expose them to the simulator stations in approximately the same amount of time and frequency as the simulator trained group. Initial analyses indicated no significant differences between the two control groups and are thus treated as a single group for this paper. However, further analyses for the effect of the educational materials are warranted. RESULTS Table 1. Driver Training Curriculum SESSIONS SECTIONS RUNTIME (min) 1 DATS INTRODUCTION Orientation Drive 3 Pre-Test Drive 6 2 LESSON 1: RESIDENTIAL Slideshow 11 Training Drive 1 6 3 Training Drive 2 6 Training Drive 3 6 Training Drive 4 6 4 LESSON 2: URBAN Slideshow 4 Training Drive 1 5 5 Training Drive 2 5 Training Drive 3 5 Training Drive 4 5 6 LESSON 3: RURAL Slideshow 9 Training Drive 1 8 7 Training Drive 2 8 Training Drive 3 8 Training Drive 4 8 8 DATS GRADUATION Orientation Drive 3 Drive 8 206

A multivariate analysis was conducted to assess driving performance factors by training groups (no/yes) across time (pre/post test), N = 67, α =.05. Performance factors included: collision avoidance accuracy (percentage of accidents avoided), intersection violations (1 amber signal and 4 stop signs), speed exceedances (posted speed limits: 20, 25, 45, and 40-70 mph), and lane keeping errors (road edge and centerline crossing counts). An interaction effect was found for training groups and time, F(4, 62) = 4.00, p =.006, η 2 =.205, suggesting that at the pre-test, no group difference was found but at the post-test, trained drivers improved more than untrained drivers. A main effect was also found for time (p <.001), suggesting that both groups improved at the post-test. Follow-up univariate tests indicated interaction effects for collision avoidance (p =.008) and speed exceedances (p =.032). A marginal effect was found for intersection violations (p =.070) and no effect for lane keeping errors (p =.864). Table 2 provides the estimated marginal means (with standard deviations) for performance measures across time and training group. Significant p values and effect sizes for pairwise comparisons between groups at test times are also provided. Driving Performance Measures Table 2. Driving performance measures across time and training groups No Training M (SD) Pre-Test Sim Training M (SD) p (η2) No Training M (SD) Sim Training M (SD) p (η2) Collision Avoid Accuracy.62 (.18).63 (.18) NS.63 (.19).80 (.15) <.001 (.19) Speed Exceedances 5.8 (2.9) 5.9 (3.3) NS 4.0 (2.7) 2.2 (1.6).001 (.15) Intersection Violations 1.2 (1.1) 1.5 (1.0) NS.91 (.75).51 (.82).060 (.05) Lane Keeping Errors 8.7 (3.5) 8.1 (3.0) NS 7.5 (2.6) 6.8 (2.3) NS NS = non significant; alpha =.05 While performance differences may be attributed to the trained group having more exposure to the simulator in general, the lack of difference in lane keeping errors between groups suggested that despite less exposure, untrained participants did not appear to have difficulty with vehicle controls or navigating the simulation environment. This was supported by subjective difficulty ratings (5 pt scale: 1=very easy, 2=easy, 3=medium, 4=difficult, 5=very difficult) collected immediately after test drives. Mann-Whitney U tests indicated no group differences for ratings at the pre-test (z = -1.26, p =.21) and post-test (z = -.06, p =.96). Wilcoxon matched-paired tests found that the trained group rated the post-test (M = 2.6, SD =.96) easier than the pre-test (M = 2.9, SD =.79), z = -2.13, p =.03. Similarly, the untrained group also rated the post- test (M = 2.7, SD = 1.13) easier than the pre-test (M = 3.2, SD =.81), z = -1.93, p =.05, despite exhibiting poorer driving performance by group from the multivariate analysis. Hazard Event Analysis A series of chi-square analyses were performed on the four hazard events (Figures 2 5) to assess whether or not group differences appeared between those students trained and untrained in the pre- and post-test drives. Obscured Stop Sign Event. As shown in Figure 6, no disproportionate number of drivers missed or stopped at the sign between training groups at pre-test. However, group differences appeared 207

% of Drivers % of Drivers % of Drivers % of Drivers PROCEEDINGS of the Sixth International Driving Symposium on Human Factors in Driver Assessment, Training and Vehicle Design for the post-test drive, with a larger percentage of drivers in the trained group (73%) who stopped at the sign than the untrained group (41%), χ 2 (1, N = 67) = 6.64, p <.05. Head-On Collision Event. As shown in Figure 7, the percentage of drivers who failed this event was about half for both groups in the pre-test. In the post-test; however, the trained group had no head-on collisions while over one third of the control group had head on collisions in the posttest drive, χ 2 (1, N = 67) = 18.58, p <.001. Vehicle Back Out Event. No significant group differences were found for both pre/post test drives. As shown in Figure 8, almost all drivers (95+%) were able to successfully avoid this particular collision event. Set with a time to collision of 3 seconds, the designed event of a vehicle backing out appeared to be too easy for beginning drivers. Vehicle Backout + Head-On Event. Provided only in the post-test drive, a larger proportion of drivers in the training group (67%) were able to avoid a collision in this situation than the control group (36%), χ 2 (1, N = 67) = 5.53, p <.05. As shown in Figures 7-9, despite high performance in the head-on collision and back-out events, 64% of untrained drivers were not able to avoid a collision when these events were combined in a novel situation. Obscured Stop Sign Event Head On Collision Event 10 12 8 6 4 No Sim Training Sim Training 55% 53% 47% 45% 41% 73% 59% 27% 10 8 6 4 No Sim Training Sim Training 55% 53% 47% 45% 64% 10 36% 2 2 Stopped Missed Stopped Missed Pre-Test Avoided Crashed Avoided Crashed Pre-Test Vehicle Backout Collision Event Vehicle Backout + Head On Collision Event 12 10 8 6 4 95% 98% No Sim Training Sim Training 95% 98% 10 8 6 4 No Sim Training Sim Training 36% 67% 64% 33% 2 5% 2% 5% 2% Avoided Crashed Avoided Crashed Pre-Test 2 Avoided Crashed 208

DISCUSSION Provided in this paper was a description of the data collected and the scenario design methods used for assessing the training efficacy for a novice driver training program. Results suggested that training groups did not differ significantly in driving performance during the pre-test drive; however, group differences emerged at the post-test. Participants in the training group provided better collision avoidance accuracy and fewer speed limit violations. A marginal difference was found for intersection violations and no difference was found for lane keeping errors. Analysis of specific hazard events also confirmed no group differences at the pre-test, but apparent during the post-test with trained drivers performing better than untrained drivers in regards to detecting a hidden stop sign and avoiding a head-on collision event. No group differences were found for the vehicle backout event, but this may have been due to an inadequate time-to-collision setting or the fact that this event was largely a simple brake reaction time event. Although both groups were able to handle the vehicle backout event, when it was combined in a novel manner with a head-on collision event and the driver had to extrapolate the event towards another driver s perspective, much fewer untrained drivers appeared to be able to avoid the situation without a crash. This suggests that with repeated exposure to backouts and head-on collisions (in training sessions), trained drivers may have used or had access to a larger mental schema of the situation and that meta-training (i.e. gaining experience in hazard perception and decision making on other tasks) may have occurred. While it can be argued that the observed performance differences were due to one group having more simulation exposure time, this paper provided evidence to the contrary. If it were the case that the untrained group merely performed worse due to lack of simulation exposure, we would expect to see differences in vehicle control demands. Instead, the data provided no differences in vehicle lane keeping, no decreases in vehicle speed to indicate higher cognitive workloads, and low subjective difficulty ratings of the scenarios. Furthermore, despite having poorer performance in the post-drive, the untrained group (similar to the trained group) rated the posttest as easier than the pre-test. Since performance feedback during the tests was removed, subjective ratings in conjunction with performance measures indicated that the untrained group exhibited a lack of hazard awareness during the test drives. Despite suggestive findings, this study has several limitations. For actual confirmation of the program s training efficacy, future studies will require cross-platform methods for assessing hazard perception abilities (e.g., driving films, on-road testing, and longitudinal crash data). Additional analyses are also required for the effect of the educational materials presented to the students as well as any inherent differences in classroom periods. Future novice driver training studies may also do well to incorporate a more mixed methods approach by incorporating focus groups or individual interviews with students/instructors to further develop salient driving scenarios and provide a more comprehensive picture of how novice drivers perceive and react to hazardous driving situations. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS 209

Systems Technology, Inc. is a privately-owned company that manufactures and distributes the STISIM Driving Simulator used in this study. This study was funded by the Center for Disease Control and Prevention (Grant No. 1R43CE001492-01). The authors are thankful for the support of Manhattan High School, Kansas and the efforts of the driver education teacher, Brad Willie (pictured in Figure 1). REFERENCES Allen, R. W., Cook, M. L., Park, G., Rosenthal, T. J., Fiorentino, D. D., & Viirre, E. (2005, January 9-13,). A PC Based Simulation System for Driver Assessment and Training. Paper presented at the TRB Annual Meeting, Washington, D.C. McKnight, A. J., & McKnight, A. S. (2003). Young novice drivers: careless or clueless? Accident Analysis and Prevention, 35(6), 921-925. Mourant, R. R., & Rockwell, T. H. (1972). Strategies of visual search by novice and experienced drivers. Human Factors, 14(4), 325-&. Pradhan, A. K., Pollatsek, A., Knodler, M., & Fisher, D. L. (2009). Can younger drivers be trained to scan for information that will reduce their risk in roadway traffic scenarios that are hard to identify as hazardous? Ergonomics, 52(6), 657-673. Terrace, S., Park, G.D. and Allen, R.W. (2010), A Novice Driver Training System, Presented at the IMAGE 2010 Conference, Scottsdale, Arizona. Underwood, G. (2007). Visual attention and the transition from novice to advanced driver. Ergonomics, 50(8), 1235-1249. 210